コーパス検索結果 (left1)
通し番号をクリックするとPubMedの該当ページを表示します
1 IQ assessed in late adolescence is a robust risk factor
2 IQ is decreased in deletion and duplication carriers, bu
3 IQ is relatively more important in predicting scores on
4 IQ motif-containing GTPase activating protein 1 (IQGAP1)
5 IQ motif-containing GTPase-activating protein 1 (IQGAP1)
6 IQ scores were available for 150 patients (60 had receiv
7 IQ slopes did not differ between groups (P = .342).
8 IQ slopes did not differ between groups (P = .509).
9 IQ was lower in the XRT group (by 12.5 points) versus th
10 IQ was lower in the XRT group (by 8.7 points) versus the
11 IQ was measured at age 18-20 as part of the Swedish mili
12 IQ, language, and motor function were measured at 7 year
13 IQ-1, a specific inhibitor of p300/beta-catenin interact
14 s costing $266 billion in the USA vs 873 000 IQ points lost and 3290 cases costing $12.6 billion in t
15 1409 individuals drawn from the top 0.0003 (IQ >170) of the population distribution of intelligence
20 ve MS (PPMS) scored a significant 4.6 to 6.9 IQ points lower than controls up to 20 years prior to fi
21 mance, as well as global conceptual ability (IQ) (P < 0.001) scores, differed significantly across th
22 al implantation of the PanOptix IOL (AcrySof IQ PanOptixTM; Alcon Research, Fort Worth, Texas, USA) p
23 A discounted lifetime value of an additional IQ point based on earnings was estimated to be pound3297
24 known to share genetic liability with ADHD: IQ, social communication, pragmatic language, and conduc
26 ations (rpart,92 = -.35, p < .001) and adult IQ (rpart,88 = .33, p = .001) even after controlling for
27 ower score (95% CI, -2.48 to -0.74) in adult IQ, a 2.07-point lower score (95% CI, -3.14 to -1.01) in
31 n Classification in MLD level 0 or 1) and an IQ of at least 85, when age at disease onset was older t
32 ogical families, the adopted siblings had an IQ 4.41 (SE = 0.75) points higher than their nonadopted
33 5 mug/L were four times as likely to have an IQ score < 80, the clinical cut-off for borderline intel
35 ; taking education costs and the value of an IQ point itself into account), and presented in terms of
38 y-one children with ADHD and thirty age- and IQ-matched typically developing (TD) controls underwent
39 functioning youths with ASDs and 19 age- and IQ-matched, typically developing youths (age range, 9-17
41 and the different correlation between BV and IQ in cases and control subjects may induce artifactual
42 difference in specificity between PQ-CSF and IQ-CSF, the latter showed a significant improvement in s
43 ia Verbal Learning Test, second edition] and IQ [Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence]) were pe
47 ounted for documented differences in sex and IQ in affected individuals with de novo mutations by mat
50 school achievement (SA) at age 16 years and IQ at ages 18 to 20 years and the deviation of that perf
51 The mean school grades at age 16 years and IQ test scores at military conscription at age 18 years.
55 , the siblings of the SZ probands had SA and IQs that did not differ from population means and were s
56 sess homologous CaM-binding motifs, known as IQ motifs in their C termini, which associate with calmo
57 ein kinase (MAPK) scaffold proteins, such as IQ motif containing GTPase activating protein 1 (IQGAP1)
60 ated using a meta-analysis of PTB-associated IQ loss, and well-established relationships of IQ loss w
64 ion had a range of intellectual ability, but IQ scores were 26 points lower than noncarrier family me
65 the majority of the error-prone TLS of dG-C8-IQ, whereas pol eta is involved primarily in its error-f
68 homology-Bin/amphiphysin/Rvs (F-BAR) Cdc15p, IQ motif containing GTPase-activating protein (IQGAP) Rn
70 ation effect; P = 6.20 x 10-3) and childhood IQ (7.9% mediation effect; P = .041) and by PEs being me
71 eight (rho = -0.155; P = .015) and childhood IQ (rho = -0.188; P = .003) were associated with the pre
72 After adjusting for maternal IQ, childhood IQ, and childhood socioeconomic status, each 5-microg/dL
77 been defined based on premorbid and current IQ, but little is known about the neuroanatomical differ
79 ficantly improved postoperative IQ and delta IQ (adjusted regression coefficient [RC] = 3.4, 95% conf
84 inly for estrogen receptor-positive disease (IQ-OR, 1.44; 95% CI, 1.16 to 1.79; P for heterogeneity =
85 ic interactions with the myosin-1C divergent IQ motifs, which are contacts that deviate from the trad
87 dent sample, we obtained proportions in each IQ-based subgroup that were similar to our previous work
88 s (n = 89) had significantly lower estimated IQ ( P < .001) and PS scores ( P = .02) compared with pa
89 by US public health insurance had estimated IQs that were significantly lower ( P < .001) than those
91 de polymorphism heritability for the extreme IQ trait was 0.33 (0.02), which is the highest so far fo
95 ition, effect sizes (Cohen's d) were 0.1 for IQ (mean difference from control 1.2, 95% CI -0.3 to 2.7
96 stimulation alone, effect sizes were 0.1 for IQ (mean difference with controls 1.7, -0.3 to 3.7), 0.3
99 elated with the neural transition frequency, IQ scores of individuals with ASD are instead predicted
100 ers and cognitive and behavioral scores from IQ testing, and parental measures of development were te
104 tive evaluation of intellectual functioning (IQ), working memory, and processing speed (PS) was condu
105 Varying levels of IQx (up to 0.29 ng/g), IQ (up to 0.93 ng/g), MeIQx (up to 0.08 ng/g), MeIQ (up
107 of the first (PQ-CSF) and second generation (IQ-CSF) RT-QuIC assays, and investigated the diagnostic
108 racts with a conserved isoleucine-glutamine (IQ) motif in the C terminus of the channel, while Ca(2+)
112 and the unexposed siblings (44% female) had IQ testing at mean ages of 10.6 and 10.9 years, respecti
114 ms (OR, 0.93 [95% CI, 0.91-0.96]) and higher IQ (OR, 1.04 [95% CI, 1.02-1.07]) in childhood compared
115 ated with less sleep disturbances and higher IQ scores in schoolchildren, 2) such relationships are n
117 dose-response relationship indicated higher IQ scores in children who always (4.80 points) or someti
118 ICC, 0.992; 95% CI: 0.986, 0.996), and IDEAL IQ and the GE 3.0-T unit (ICC, 0.966; 95% CI: 0.939, 0.9
120 motional problems, and cognitive impairment (IQ score <80) during childhood (ages 6, 11, and 15 years
124 omic status in adulthood and with changes in IQ and socioeconomic mobility between childhood and midl
127 phrenia and control participants, changes in IQ were negatively correlated with changes in lateral ve
128 n greater blood lead levels and a decline in IQ and socioeconomic status from childhood to adulthood
130 ars at diagnosis are at risk for deficits in IQ and PS in the absence of cranial radiation, regardles
135 The pooled weight mean difference (WMD) in IQs between NBW and LBW individuals was 10 (95% CI 9.26-
138 Diagnosis of a psychotic disorder, initial IQ, longitudinal IQ trajectory, and timing of the last p
140 d a hypomethylated region mapping to Iqgap2 (IQ motif-containing GTPase activating protein 2) and F2r
143 ith 3 different types of hydrophobic lenses: IQ SN60WF (494 eyes) and ReSTOR SN6AD1 (169 eyes) (Alcon
144 were independent of age, educational level, IQ, APOE genotype, subjective memory complaints, vascula
145 psychotic disorder, initial IQ, longitudinal IQ trajectory, and timing of the last psychiatric assess
148 es showed that the association between lower IQ and schizophrenia is not the result of shared familia
149 were more likely to have borderline or lower IQ compared with children of mothers consuming <100 mg/d
150 r had a mean (SE) of 4.4 (0.72) points lower IQ than those without severe disorder (P < .001), and th
154 8 individuals from the top 0.0003 ( 170 mean IQ) of the population distribution of intelligence and 8
158 suggests that observed declines in measured IQ may not be a direct result of marijuana exposure but
160 subclinical hypothyroidism trial, the median IQ score of the children was 97 (95% confidence interval
161 In the hypothyroxinemia trial, the median IQ score was 94 (95% CI, 91 to 95) in the levothyroxine
162 o polybrominated diphenyl ethers (11 million IQ points lost and 43 000 cases costing $266 billion in
163 redictive power was similar to the TC model (IQ-OR, 1.45; 95% CI, 1.21 to 1.73; mC, 0.55), but SNP88
164 dings on the Full-Scale IQ (FSIQ), Nonverbal IQ, and Verbal IQ; the presence of ASD or other DSM-IV d
169 at factors influencing mild ID (lowest 3% of IQ distribution) were similar to those influencing IQ in
170 actors influencing severe ID (lowest 0.5% of IQ distribution) differ from those influencing mild ID o
171 er performance on a school-age assessment of IQ, a measure whose relevance for occupational success i
176 zed that some of the missing heritability of IQ might lie hidden in the human leukocyte antigen (HLA)
178 gical functioning (mean [SD]) on measures of IQ (81.22 [15.97] vs 91.28 [14.31]; Cohen d, -0.70), ver
179 nificant impairment on mean (SD) measures of IQ (95.25 [16.58] vs 100.45 [14.77]; Cohen d, -0.22; P =
180 parents and adoptees, and direct measures of IQ to measure symptoms of autism spectrum disorder, inat
182 Our results confirm the high sensitivity of IQ-CSF for detecting human prions with a sub-optimal sen
185 healthy comparison subjects group-matched on IQ, gender, and age performed a passive avoidance task w
186 healthy comparison subjects group-matched on IQ, gender, and age performed a passive avoidance task w
187 ated the causal effect of DNA methylation on IQ using the offspring genotype at sites close to the me
188 attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder or IQ, age range was associated with gray matter difference
190 nity binding site, which leaves the original IQ-motif exposed, thereby destabilizing the lever arm.
191 nfluenced by environmental factors, parental IQs and other factors contribute to residual confounding
193 SD; 95% CI, -0.69 to -0.40), and Performance IQ (-0.41 SD; 95% CI, -0.56 to -0.27); these SD scores c
194 primary outcomes were verbal and performance IQ scores on the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of
195 l intelligence quotient (IQ) and performance IQ scores over a period of 5 years were significantly su
196 54) were analysed for verbal and performance IQ with WPPSI-III and 315 (iodine group, n=159; placebo
198 CI -2.9 to 1.5; p=0.77), and for performance IQ were 97.5 (12.5) in the iodine group and 99.1 (13.4)
200 ion was observed with treatment arm (placebo IQ-OR, 1.46; 95% CI, 1.13 to 1.87; tamoxifen IQ-OR, 1.25
203 est NPA significantly improved postoperative IQ and delta IQ (adjusted regression coefficient [RC] =
209 ramide signaling on an ERK scaffold protein, IQ motif containing GTPase activating protein 1 (IQGAP1)
210 found for Full Scale intelligence quotient (IQ) (-0.52 SD; 95% CI, -0.68 to -0.37), Verbal IQ (-0.54
211 s, gait disturbances, intelligence quotient (IQ) and adaptive function, as well as paternal and mater
213 full-scale or global intelligence quotient (IQ) and performance IQ scores over a period of 5 years w
215 age 4 and full scale intelligence quotient (IQ) from the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence
216 We compared change in intelligence quotient (IQ) over time in pediatric patients with brain tumors tr
217 was driven mainly by intelligence quotient (IQ) points loss and intellectual disability due to polyb
219 <2500 g) have a lower intelligence quotient (IQ) than those with normal birth weights (NBW, >/=2500 g
227 isk overall (interquartile range odds ratio [IQ-OR], 1.37; 95% CI, 1.14 to 1.66; mC, 0.55), but mainl
228 rded SA and 106187 individuals with recorded IQ born in Sweden between January 1, 1972, and December
229 between increasing psychosis RPS and reduced IQ (matrix reasoning: corrected P = .003 for RPS model 2
232 n inverted-J-shaped association with child's IQ at age 7 years, with a peak difference (vs. undetecta
234 nship between caffeine intake and children's IQ at 5.5 years (-.94 [95% confidence interval = -1.70,
235 caffeine intake during pregnancy, children's IQ at age 5.5, and individual and family characteristics
236 ognitive aptitude is calculated from the SA, IQ, and educational attainment in biological relatives.
239 ered standardized scores for both Full Scale IQ and Verbal Comprehension, Perceptual Reasoning, Worki
241 cantly negatively associated with Full Scale IQ, and with all Index scores (except Processing Speed).
245 nce interval (CI): -3.9, -0.5] in Full-Scale IQ and 2.9 points (95% CI: -4.4, -1.3) in Verbal Compreh
248 we observed similar decrements in Full-Scale IQ with each standard deviation increase of use for two
249 ental domains: cognitive ability (Full-Scale IQ), social behavior (Social Responsiveness Scale), and
253 ith and without these genetic events on sex, IQ, and age before comparing them on multiple behavioral
254 of high-functioning ASD males and age-/sex-/IQ-matched controls, we first identified age-associated
259 IQ-OR, 1.46; 95% CI, 1.13 to 1.87; tamoxifen IQ-OR, 1.25; 95% CI, 0.96 to 1.64; P for heterogeneity =
265 (MRI) brain volumes were compared among the IQ-based subgroups using analysis of covariance with int
268 h germanium oxide-based PET/CT scanners, the IQ with 5-ring detector blocks has the highest overall p
269 ve in vitro binding assays, we show that the IQ domain of IQGAP1 is both necessary and sufficient for
270 nalysis with pure proteins revealed that the IQ region of IQGAP1 binds directly to the intracellular
271 rmore, we determined that CaM binding to the IQ motif was required for channel function, indicating t
273 n showed that the interaction occurs via the IQ domain of IQGAP1 and the TEAD-binding domain of YAP.
276 -sensing protein, is constitutively bound to IQ domains of the C termini of human Kv7 (hKv7, KCNQ) ch
278 ves: To confirm previous findings related to IQ-based subgroups of patients with schizophrenia in an
280 ) (-0.52 SD; 95% CI, -0.68 to -0.37), Verbal IQ (-0.54 SD; 95% CI, -0.69 to -0.40), and Performance I
281 ler discrepancy between nonverbal and verbal IQ and a greater likelihood of having achieved fluent la
282 ociated with decreased full-scale and verbal IQ scores compared with duplication carriers without the
283 ll-Scale IQ (FSIQ), Nonverbal IQ, and Verbal IQ; the presence of ASD or other DSM-IV diagnoses; BMI;
285 nd was associated with impairments in verbal IQ, attention, executive function, language and visuospa
286 ld not be explained by differences in verbal IQ, intracranial volume, anxiety/depression, or attentio
287 exposures were associated with lower verbal IQ in minimally adjusted models; but after adjustment fo
291 Moreover, in contrast to the controls whose IQ is correlated with the neural transition frequency, I
292 ods: through static estimates of cohort-wide IQ deficits and through dynamic economy-wide effects usi
294 d eye movement stage 2 sleep, correlate with IQ and are thought to promote long-term potentiation and
295 ll tissue volumes in infancy correlated with IQ (r >/= 0.35, P < .05) and language (r >/= 0.29, P < .
297 nction; however, a negative correlation with IQ at age 11 years (beta=-0.08, Z=-3.3, P=0.001) was obs
299 ygous for a 2 base pair (bp) deletion within IQ calmodulin-binding motif-containing protein-1 (IQCB1)
300 ssociated with IQ decline or impairment, yet IQ slopes did not differ between the PBRT and XRT groups
WebLSDに未収録の専門用語(用法)は "新規対訳" から投稿できます。