戻る
「早戻しボタン」を押すと検索画面に戻ります。

今後説明を表示しない

[OK]

コーパス検索結果 (1語後でソート)

通し番号をクリックするとPubMedの該当ページを表示します
1 ance of opioid withdrawal (conditioned place aversion).
2 r 2; VGluT2), which play roles in reward and aversion.
3 tivation caused neither place preference nor aversion.
4 as icv TTR did not cause a conditioned taste aversion.
5  behavior when it was guided by innate taste aversion.
6 tificial activation is sufficient to mediate aversion.
7 al for imprinted, but not for adult-learned, aversion.
8 ed rewards, is associated with loss and risk aversion.
9 d by their efferent targets, code reward and aversion.
10 essary and sufficient to mediate behavioural aversion.
11 reward, whereas stimulating D2 MSNs produces aversion.
12 ral synchrony and spatial representations of aversion.
13 uppresses activity of a glomerulus wired for aversion.
14 ular control of opioid systems in reward and aversion.
15 nd sufficient for stress-induced anxiety and aversion.
16 dum hedonic cold spot led to real-time place aversion.
17 f a saliency spectrum ranging from reward to aversion.
18  nocifensive responses and conditional place aversion.
19  area (VTA) neurons play roles in reward and aversion.
20 sion and probability weighting to model risk aversion.
21 olerance, or conditioned-place preference or aversion.
22 g effects, which is contingent on their loss aversion.
23  the balance between drug-induced reward and aversion.
24 lutionary explanation for the origin of risk aversion.
25 er volume in this region exhibited less risk aversion.
26 eptors in mesohabenular stimulation-elicited aversion.
27 sponse speed, or promote a conditioned place aversion.
28 al area (VTA) plays roles in both reward and aversion.
29 at did not depend on sweetness perception or aversion.
30 and stress-related behaviors, such as social aversion.
31  betting, and impulsivity, measured as delay aversion.
32 h loss-sensitivity, and thus diminishes loss aversion.
33  whereas TRPM8 neurons are required for cold aversion.
34 nsitivity to losses than gains known as loss aversion.
35 ved in motor control, resulted in defects in aversion.
36 gustatory system are responsible for glucose aversion.
37 glutamate/Homer2 expression in MA preference/aversion.
38  (gain only)-were exclusively driven by risk aversion.
39 eption while activation of LC(:PFC) produced aversion.
40 five sessions that tested conditioned flavor aversion.
41 relin signaling can both inhibit and enhance aversion.
42  risk aversion but equivalent levels of loss aversion.
43 health, and individual prosociality and risk aversion.
44  reward as a measure of an individual's risk aversion.
45 secretion, body weight, or conditioned taste aversion.
46 d not induce conditioned place preference or aversion.
47 osite limb, as assessed by conditioned place aversion.
48 ecision-making tasks assessing risk and loss aversion.
49  the naloxone-precipitated MWD induced place aversion.
50  mesoaccumbens glutamatergic fibers promotes aversion.
51 ion and maintenance of taste preferences and aversions.
52 there thus be an evolutionary origin to risk aversion?
53 re absolutely required for conditioned taste aversion, a learned behavior.
54                  Hypotheses include inequity aversion, a moral sense that inequality is intrinsically
55 ateral habenula (LHb) is involved in reward, aversion, addiction and depression through descending in
56 ty and to find a way between desire for, and aversion against, eating.
57 estation of hyper-cooperation and inequality aversion, allowing exploration of high-quality, hard-to-
58 ndrome, a disorder characterized by eye-gaze aversion, among other social and cognitive deficits.
59 s, produced dose-dependent conditioned place aversion and a reduction in the above optical ICSS in Vg
60       In particular, it is not clear whether aversion and attraction bidirectionally modulate a share
61 ation of neural circuitry that mediates odor aversion and attraction will provide key insights into h
62 hila melanogaster, displays robust olfactory aversion and attraction, but how these behaviors are exe
63       There was no relationship between loss aversion and delay discounting across the sample, nor in
64 bjects with experimental tasks assaying loss aversion and delay discounting.
65                         Two hypotheses, gaze aversion and gaze indifference, are commonly cited to ex
66 :PFC) exacerbated spontaneous pain, produced aversion and increased anxiety-like behaviour.
67 ral habenula (LHb) is involved in reward and aversion and is reciprocally connected with dopamine (DA
68 d allergy, as they showed a reversal of food aversion and lower IgE production after 5 weeks of induc
69  and circuit logic of predator-driven innate aversion and may serve as a valuable model for studying
70 attenuated the peripheral CGRP-induced light aversion and motility behaviors.
71 t-inducible stimulation of acute pain, place aversion and optogenetically mediated reductions in with
72 ther with the propensity to induce reward or aversion and physical dependence.
73 mists also make use of concepts such as loss aversion and probability weighting to model risk aversio
74 contributors to the balance between nicotine aversion and reward.
75 inancial decision-making task measuring loss aversion and risk attitude.
76                        These are termed loss aversion and risk aversion, respectively.
77      However, the role of DA in KOR-mediated aversion and stress remains divisive as recent studies h
78  in regulating expression of ethanol-induced aversion and suggest a mechanism for its role in modulat
79 ic mice also demonstrated a reversal of food aversion and sustained visceral fat after 5 weeks of all
80 s, giving rise to phenomena such as betrayal aversion and taboo-tradeoffs, and specific patterns of f
81 were able to reliably estimate risk and loss aversion and their respective contribution to gambling d
82 ha3, beta4, and alpha5 receptors in nicotine aversion and withdrawal have been established, the cellu
83 prevented naloxone-induced conditioned place aversions and decreases in sensitivity to brain stimulat
84 gests behavioral factors (eg, increased risk aversion) and/or allocation inefficiencies may have play
85  variational, maxmin, constant absolute risk aversion, and constant relative risk aversion utilities.
86 A), alters BLA neuronal activity, conditions aversion, and increases anxiety-like behavior.
87 capone exerted selective effects on inequity aversion, and not on other computational components such
88 b) nuclei play important roles in processing aversion, and recent work has focused on the critical in
89 isrupted anxiety and mPFC representations of aversion, and reduced theta synchrony in a pathway-, fre
90 ecific functional roles in mediating reward, aversion, and stress.
91 from a single receptor can block innate odor aversion, and that instinctive behavioral responses to o
92           Patients with CKD suffer from food aversion, anorexia, and malnutrition.
93 references changed substantially toward risk aversion as reward accumulated within a context, and blo
94 en arm of an elevated plus maze, and an odor aversion associated with early-life footshocks.
95 osing directions on olfactory attraction and aversion at the level of the first synapse.
96 icted by interindividual differences in risk-aversion attitudes expressed during the gambling task.
97 s to DA neurons that might impact the reward/aversion balance of alcohol attributes, which may contri
98          Empathy is more effective than risk-aversion because when infectious individuals change beha
99 d gene expression and eliminated the feeding-aversion behavior resulting from contact with the analog
100 ns, with corresponding changes in attraction/aversion behavior.
101                             Finally, a light-aversion behavioral assay to assess functionality reveal
102 -dependent modulation of morphine reward and aversion behaviors.
103 ing uncertainty, eating: between craving and aversion, being different and professional help as a fur
104 dition, participants exhibited a strong risk aversion bias; strikingly, this bias reversed in the rea
105 anxious participants exhibited enhanced risk aversion but equivalent levels of loss aversion.
106 l preference, are dispensable for egg-laying aversion but essential for movement aversion of UV.
107              Choices indicated loss and risk aversion but unbiased estimation of probabilities.
108 ot produce a conditioned place preference or aversion, but elicited CB1 receptor-mediated antinocicep
109 d novel taste learning and conditioned taste aversion, but not memory retrieval.
110 lso prevented the establishment of the place aversion by a CB1 mechanism of action.
111 2-mesohabenular neurons that plays a role in aversion by activating LHb glutamatergic receptors.
112                           By contrast, error-aversion certainty (certainty of not making an error) re
113 ehavioral economics, including inertia, loss aversion, choice overload, and relative social ranking.
114 ve losses, whereas individuals with low loss aversion choked for large prospective losses but not for
115 sregulated activity of the mesocorticolimbic aversion circuit may be etiologically related to errors
116 ne predominantly shapes the so-called reward/aversion circuitry, with major influence on negative aff
117                                Risk and loss aversion correlated with each other in the depressed pat
118 c neurons was required for conditioned place aversion (CPA) in mice.
119 tion of sexual behavior in a conditioned sex aversion (CSA) paradigm where mating is paired with illn
120  sensitivity to MA-induced conditioned taste aversion (CTA) and hypothermia.
121               Here, we use conditioned taste aversion (CTA) in rats, a cortically dependent learning
122                            Conditioned taste aversion (CTA) is a phenomenon in which an individual fo
123 ly regarded as integral to conditioned taste aversion (CTA) retention, a link that has been primarily
124 sed versus no-choice-based conditioned taste aversion (CTA) tasks in rats.
125 ficacy was confirmed using conditioned taste aversion (CTA) tests.
126  kg(-1) 20% ethanol, i.p.) conditioned taste aversion (CTA) to saccharin taste.
127  and after ethanol-induced conditioned taste aversion (CTA) to saccharin.
128 memory representations for conditioned taste aversion (CTA).
129 quisition and retention of conditioned taste aversions (CTAs) in rodents, but large lesions in this a
130 hould switch between risk proneness and risk aversion depending on state and circumstances, especiall
131 c silencing of the LH-to-LHb pathway impairs aversion-driven escape behaviors.
132  by the trade name Antabuse), an old alcohol-aversion drug that has been shown to be effective agains
133 ulators, cortisol and noradrenaline, on loss aversion during financial decision making.
134        We show that disadvantageous inequity aversion emerged across all populations by middle childh
135 imple binary-choice model, we show that risk aversion emerges by natural selection if reproductive ri
136                             However, whether aversion encoding requires these neural circuits to ulti
137 ver LHb neurons monosynaptically innervating aversion-encoding midbrain GABA cells.
138  contrast to their role in inflammatory pain aversion, EP3 receptors on serotonergic cells were dispe
139             The existence of innate predator aversion evoked by predator-derived chemostimuli called
140 ccumbens glutamatergic fibers lack GABA, the aversion evoked by their photoactivation depended on glu
141             We test the hypothesis that risk aversion extends the length of stay in the dwelling and,
142 ocomotion and responsivity, and reduced risk-aversion/fearfulness.
143 havior showed that citalopram increased harm aversion for both self and others, while levodopa reduce
144 mental oxygen also induces conditioned place aversion, for which Gucy1b2 and Trpc2 are required.
145 ploration of the action space for successful aversion formation and show how it predicts foraging beh
146                       Long-lasting imprinted aversion has a critical period in the first larval stage
147 trust between dissimilar users and that risk aversion has an inverse relationship with trust given hi
148                   A marked bias towards risk aversion has been observed in nearly every species teste
149 urophysiological evidence suggests that loss aversion has its origins in relatively ancient neural ci
150 bability, sometimes called risk proneness or aversion, has been assumed to be static.
151 R-mediated dysphoria, assessed in rodents as aversion, has recently been attributed to the activation
152 ation analysis revealed that behavioral loss aversion hindered performance via the influence of ventr
153                 The results falsify the gaze aversion hypothesis; instead, at the time of initial dia
154                                Reducing risk aversion, improving allocation, and more often pumping l
155 ntegrating information about both reward and aversion in a common currency.
156                  We previously reported work aversion in an effortful T-maze task following a binge e
157 on, constipation, and displayed no reward or aversion in CPP/CPA assays, suggesting that analogs migh
158  body weight, or causing a conditioned taste aversion in mice lacking neuronal GLP1R.
159 ripheral taste sensitivity underlies glucose aversion in multiple GA populations.
160   Here, we show that female flies exhibit UV aversion in response to their egg-laying demand.
161 own about the mechanisms underlying cannabis aversion in rodents.
162 ative to either drug by itself, reduced loss aversion in the absence of an effect on risk attitude or
163 trategy, was positively correlated with loss aversion in the depressed patients, also implicating imp
164                     We hypothesize that risk aversion in this gamble is beneficial as an adaptation t
165                           Behaviorally, risk aversion increased across age groups, with adults unifor
166 endent; as the animals became "poorer," risk aversion increased, a finding incompatible with some mod
167 s cognate ligand from blocking predator odor aversion, indicating that the blocking requires sensory
168                          We found that place aversion induced by inflammatory pain depends on prostag
169 able for acute nociceptive behaviors and for aversion induced by thermal pain or a kappa opioid recep
170      We tested the role of CRF in modulating aversion-induced changes in dopamine concentration and c
171 glutamatergic input to nAcc shown to mediate aversion instead of reward, and the first pathway shown
172 e that outcome anticipation and ensuing loss aversion involve multiple neural systems, showing functi
173                                         Risk aversion is a common behavior universal to humans and an
174                                      Glucose-aversion is a heritable trait that evolved in a number o
175                                         Loss aversion is a well-known behavioral regularity in financ
176 ur framework implies that the degree of risk aversion is determined by the stochastic nature of repro
177                                         Risk aversion is one of the most basic assumptions of economi
178                                         Loss aversion is particularly sensitive to how the decision i
179              Here, we used conditioned taste aversion learning in the rat model, wherein animals asso
180 city onto DA neurons is causally involved in aversion learning is unknown.
181  first to reject it (i.e., conditioned taste aversion learning) and then to enjoy it again (i.e., ext
182 es ingestive behavior without inducing taste aversion may open the possibility of a therapeutic appli
183                               Deficient harm aversion may underlie instrumental and reactive aggressi
184 teral habenula (LHb), which is implicated in aversion-mediated learning, show accelerated escalation
185 egulation of drug-seeking behaviours through aversion-mediated learning.
186 egulation of drug-seeking behaviours through aversion-mediated learning.
187 tenuated the extinction of established taste aversion memory without altering the initial associative
188 this is dissociable from AIC-dependent taste aversion memory.
189  the adjacent insular cortex-dependent taste aversion memory.
190 ther further modifications could reduce risk aversion, more efficiently allocate resources, and incre
191 tly on the habenula and its critical role in aversion, negative-reward and drug dependence.
192 t atrophy in an interoceptive, pain-related (aversion) network exhibited the most severe lack of soci
193 ocedure but did not affect conditioned place aversion, nor did JQ1 alone induce conditioned aversion
194 eep arousal (locus coeruleus) and preference/aversion (nucleus accumbens) demonstrate the unique capa
195                      Acute MWD induced place aversion occurs when naloxone is administered 24 h follo
196 conditioned on the levels of risk acceptance/aversion of decision makers, and show how to extend the
197                       The unpleasantness and aversion of overdrinking is associated with activation i
198  retina suggest that egg-laying and movement aversion of UV are both mediated by the inner (R7) and n
199 g-laying aversion but essential for movement aversion of UV.
200           Second, they also exhibit movement aversion of UV: positional tracking of single females su
201            First, females exhibit egg-laying aversion of UV: they prefer to lay eggs on dark sites wh
202            Superhydrophobicity stems from an aversion of water for the hydrophobic surface texture, s
203                         We observe that risk aversion only evolves when the gamble is a rare event th
204  a stereotyped behavioural response, such as aversion or attraction.
205 ersion, nor did JQ1 alone induce conditioned aversion or preference.
206 nal activity and thus contribute to nicotine aversion or reward.
207 athway, or a kappa-opiate receptor-dependent aversion pathway, directly within the ventral tegmental
208                 Notably, mice devoid of salt-aversion pathways show unimpeded, continuous attraction
209 ntrolling dissociable subcortical reward and aversion pathways.
210 fairness decisions: disadvantageous inequity aversion (peer receives more than self) and advantageous
211  a vital role in their host seeking and risk aversion processes.
212           The TPNs essential for conditioned aversion project to the superior lateral protocerebrum (
213 ases of choice-outcome anticipation and loss aversion provided inconsistent results, showing either b
214 ynorphinerigic cells regulate preference and aversion provides insight into motivated behaviors that
215  measures of reward, drive, mood, and sexual aversion, providing functional significance.
216 lated with the extinction of the conditioned aversion (R(2) = 0.58, p < 0.005).
217 s on the human brain, particularly on reward/aversion regions implicated in addiction, such as the nu
218 nd context-independent forms of rewarding or aversion-related emotional associative memories.
219 ns occurred in connectional patterns of pain/aversion-related nodes, including the mu receptor-enrich
220 se results suggest that kappa opioid-induced aversion requires regulation of VTA dopaminergic neuron
221 indicate that HA-NMDAR modulators can reduce aversion-resistant alcohol drinking, and support testing
222 ion of Syt2 expression in the mPFC increased aversion-resistant alcohol drinking, supporting a mechan
223                    Systemic D-serine reduced aversion-resistant alcohol drinking, without altering co
224 rsolateral striatum also selectively reduced aversion-resistant alcohol drinking.
225 impact of D-serine and D-cycloserine on this aversion-resistant alcohol intake (that persists despite
226 eased alcohol self-administration, increased aversion-resistant alcohol intake and enhanced stress-in
227 er mPFC- and insula-to-NAcore inputs sustain aversion-resistant alcohol intake.
228                                              Aversion-resistant intake was associated with a new type
229 lecular mechanisms and pathways that sustain aversion-resistant intake.
230      These are termed loss aversion and risk aversion, respectively.
231 hus directly implicating the Aedae-KR in the aversion response.
232 (DA) neurons have been implicated in reward, aversion, salience, cognition, and several neuropsychiat
233 ve behavior in wild-type CD1 mice similar to aversion seen previously after central (intracerebrovent
234 es more than self) and advantageous inequity aversion (self receives more than a peer).
235  slaf = ball, slaflaf = balls), the doubling aversion shifts into a reliable (morphological) preferen
236  of choice such as motor impulsivity or loss aversion, suggesting a direct and specific influence of
237 enorhabditis elegans leads to a long-lasting aversion that requires distinct sets of neurons for the
238      The allergic C57BL/6 mice exhibited EWS aversion that was associated with less visceral fat and
239 ons and that this signaling sequence induced aversion through GABA-mediated inhibition of dopaminergi
240 e found that systemic inflammation triggered aversion through MyD88-dependent activation of the brain
241 rea (VTA), a region implicated in reward and aversion, thus providing a candidate neural substrate fo
242  central sensitization and conditioned place aversion, thus providing a novel paradigm to investigate
243 oth a TAAR ligand and a common odorant block aversion to a predator odor, indicating that this abilit
244 ared with the other groups exhibited greater aversion to both risk and loss during gambles involving
245                    Sensory hypersensitivity (aversion to certain sounds, touch, etc., or increased ab
246 nd report that the fly displays strong taste aversion to common carboxylic acids.
247 raft survival, accepting risk, and desperate aversion to dialysis), guilt and responsibility (jeopard
248                                          The aversion to feeding elicited by analog 1728 suggests tha
249                         They tend to have an aversion to foods rich in fat.
250 s that are calorie- and protein-dense and an aversion to foods that are poisonous or toxic.
251 rodent pain assay based on conditioned place aversion to formalin injection, an inflammatory noxious
252 f the dorsal raphe nucleus failed to form an aversion to formalin-induced pain, as did mice lacking t
253  have rapidly evolved an adaptive behavioral aversion to glucose (a phagostimulant component of baits
254                                           An aversion to harming others is a core component of human
255 ong many mammals, giving birth leads from an aversion to infant stimuli to irresistible attraction.
256 sly observed that most people show a greater aversion to inflicting pain on others than themselves.
257                            Here we show that aversion to information gathering can be beneficial even
258 p cooperating even if conditions change, and aversion to information gathering helps to interact pref
259 ed conditioned flavor (i.e., taste and odor) aversion to intravenously self-administered (IVSA) nicot
260 wever, this is not the only reason for their aversion to light.
261 as alpha6 KO showed both a conditioned place aversion to lithium chloride as well as CPP to palatable
262 pensity to take risks rather than a stronger aversion to losses.
263 nes focus on risk, often assuming negligible aversion to medication, yet most patients discontinue pr
264 y about the decision outcome (e.g., risk) or aversion to negative outcomes (e.g., loss).
265        The authors connect conservatism with aversion to negativity via the tendentious use of the la
266 ychopathic traits correlated negatively with aversion to pain for both self and others, in line with
267 ceptor activation elicited conditioned place aversion to pain via inhibition of serotonergic neurons.
268 cidal behavior is associated with heightened aversion to risk and loss, which might produce negative
269 tion that despite the effects of warnings on aversion to smoking, intention to quit smoking is an inv
270 r was sufficient to produce pups' subsequent aversion to that odor.
271 stimulation, mice showed a conditioned place aversion to the chamber that was previously associated w
272  just in particular research areas but in an aversion to the explanatory depth available from politic
273 s non-aggressors develop a conditioned place aversion to the intruder-paired context.
274  with delta opioid-induced seizure activity, aversion to the kappa drug U50, 488H, or alpha2-mediated
275 ession in dopaminergic neurons blocked place aversion to the KOR agonist U50,488.
276 g with pathogenic bacteria induces a learned aversion to the smell of the pathogen, a behavioral plas
277 to pathogenic bacteria results in persistent aversion to those bacterial odors, whereas adult exposur
278 e to acoustic masking and/or distraction and aversion to traffic noise.
279 wever, this bias could be driven by enhanced aversion to uncertainty about the decision outcome (e.g.
280 ever, was mediated by winter severity, where aversion to well pads decreased as winter severity incre
281  the paradox of how flies maintain reflexive aversion to your approaching swatter, whilst tolerating
282  experiences that shape odor preferences and aversions to explore developmental plasticity in circuit
283 prey and eyespots are large, and that innate aversion toward eyespots is conditional.
284 te risk aversion, and constant relative risk aversion utilities.
285           U69,593-mediated conditioned place aversion was blocked by intra-mPFC nor-BNI microinjectio
286  the degree of participants' behavioral loss aversion was correlated with their susceptibility to cho
287                          No evidence of risk aversion was found.
288           By contrast, advantageous inequity aversion was more variable, emerging in three population
289 y, we demonstrated that inflammation-induced aversion was not an indirect consequence of fever or ano
290 induce memory given that a conditioned taste aversion was obtained for a novel taste, presented just
291                                   Medication aversion was quantified as the gain in lifespan required
292 l, ENaC, but the cellular substrate for salt aversion was unknown.
293 g in the LHb contributing to ethanol-induced aversion, we recorded neural firing in the LHb of freely
294 nd loss frames: individuals with higher loss aversion were susceptible to choking for large prospecti
295 mixed)-could be driven by both loss and risk aversion, whereas choices on the other type-featuring on
296 ation of the RMTg produced conditioned place aversion, whereas cocaine-induced avoidance behaviors in
297 red with the expression of conditioned place aversions while leaving intact cocaine-induced place pre
298 othetical rewards, suggesting increased loss aversion with real monetary rewards.
299                      Here we focused on loss aversion with the aim to address interindividual differe
300  and associated hormonal changes affect loss aversion, yet the underlying neuroendocrine mechanisms a

WebLSDに未収録の専門用語(用法)は "新規対訳" から投稿できます。
 
Page Top