コーパス検索結果 (1語後でソート)
通し番号をクリックするとPubMedの該当ページを表示します
1 ference to studies of people who are deaf or bilingual.
2 ing the results reported for older and early bilinguals.
3 ching the pattern observed in Chinese/French bilinguals.
4 rom the perspective of grammatical gender in bilinguals.
5 n made predictions for both monolinguals and bilinguals (33 patients; 18 males and 15 females; age at
7 e to answer this question because proficient bilinguals activate the same brain regions irrespective
10 ther, our results predict that the so-called bilingual advantage should be limited to individuals who
14 al organization in adults, in early and late bilinguals and in people who have acquired language thro
18 experience-related tuning of attention, the bilingual auditory system becomes highly efficient in au
19 alibrated to their own experiences: Tests of bilingual babies reveal that an infant's sociolinguistic
20 l connectivity was observed for simultaneous bilinguals between the left and right IFG, as well as be
24 only in native signers (hearing, ASL-English bilinguals) but not in those who acquired ASL after pube
25 merican monolingual children and 12 Japanese bilingual children with second-order false-belief story
29 control and the default mode networks in the bilingual, compared with the monolingual, AD patients.
30 executive function ability, suggesting that bilinguals compensated for lower levels of cognitive con
31 2, younger and older adult monolinguals and bilinguals completed the same perceptual task-switching
34 cial to the comprehension of a fully natural bilingual conversation recorded "in the wild." Our resul
35 al to evaluate the efficacy of exposure to a bilingual, culturally targeted website, Informate, for i
37 y, we tested a single group of Welsh-English bilinguals engaged in a nonverbal conflict resolution ta
40 dings introduce a new level of plasticity in bilingual executive control dependent on fast changing l
42 anxiety disorder were more prevalent in the bilingual group assigned to English than in the group in
44 volumetric measurements of HG revealed that bilinguals have, on average, larger Heschl's gyri than m
47 ed at cortical activation in Spanish-English bilinguals in response to phonological competition eithe
54 mes from research looking at early and older bilingual individuals who have been using both their fir
56 In a naturalistic eye-tracking procedure, bilingual infants were more accurate at recognizing obje
60 tended to be over-optimistic when predicting bilingual language outcomes: our bilingual patients tend
63 , Branigan & Pickering (B&P) briefly discuss bilingual language representation, focusing primarily on
64 ctural priming drives real-life phenomena of bilingual language use beyond the priming of unilingual
72 king evidence from infancy to adulthood that bilinguals monitor their languages for efficient compreh
75 social programs and interventions to support bilingual or multilingual education and the maintenance
76 predicting bilingual language outcomes: our bilingual patients tended to have poorer language skills
81 nt 1 ERPs were recorded while French-English bilinguals read pure language lists of French and Englis
83 e implicit access to the first language when bilinguals read words exclusively in their second langua
84 glish monolingual and 16 early Welsh-English bilingual readers undergoing event-related brain potenti
85 Here, we show that balanced Welsh-English bilinguals reading in English unconsciously apply a morp
92 ched for disease duration (45 German-Italian bilingual speakers and 40 monolingual speakers) were inc
93 rained with monolingual patient data, are to bilingual stroke patients who had been ordinarily reside
96 cond languages acquired in adulthood ('late' bilingual subjects) are spatially separated from native
97 ge acquisition stage of development ('early' bilingual subjects), native and second languages tend to
102 lated with age of acquisition for sequential bilinguals; the earlier the second language was acquired
103 (second language learned after age 5 years) bilinguals using a seed-based resting-state MRI approach
104 aled higher fractional anisotropy values for bilinguals vs. monolinguals in several WM tracts that ha
105 tions of German-English and Japanese-English bilinguals, we suggest that the left caudate plays a uni
106 mage in the same sets of regions, though the bilinguals were more sensitive than the monolinguals.
107 ls and a group of proficient Spanish-English bilinguals were presented with a multiple-deviant oddbal
110 d by words determines language activation in bilinguals, where potentially disturbing stimuli trigger
111 fMRI to look at simultaneous and sequential bilinguals who differed only in age of acquisition, and
112 Heschl's gyrus by comparing Spanish-Catalan bilinguals who have been exposed to two languages since
113 ask matched those observed in Chinese/French bilinguals who have had continual exposure to Chinese si
114 en language and cognitive control regions in bilinguals who learned their two languages simultaneousl
116 visual modality, and (iii) normally hearing bilinguals who were native signers of ASL and speakers o
118 dence showing cross-language permeability in bilingual word recognition, a phenomena that was predict
119 stigated whether young, highly immersed late bilinguals would also show structural effects in the WM
WebLSDに未収録の専門用語(用法)は "新規対訳" から投稿できます。