コーパス検索結果 (1語後でソート)
通し番号をクリックするとPubMedの該当ページを表示します
1 e in the degree to which neurons become more binocular.
2 inearly at two stages, one monocular and one binocular.
4 sessed using a linear regression model, with binocular 10-2 VF sensitivity as the independent variabl
5 te NEI VFQ-25 score was associated with both binocular 24-2 (beta = 1.95; 95% CI, 0.47-3.43; P = .01)
9 ular channels using untilted or non-oriented binocular adaptation patterns, we controlled the perceiv
13 ased disparity tuning was most pronounced in binocular and ipsilaterally biased neurons, which are th
14 for baseline VA, the difference between the binocular and patching groups was -2.7 letters (95% CI:
17 score was correlated significantly with all binocular approximations of VF, with r values ranging fr
19 Associations between binocular tests and binocular approximations to represent binocular visual f
20 ship of binocular visual function tests with binocular approximations using data from the Collaborati
21 nocular VA showed stronger correlations with binocular approximations, with r values ranging from 0.6
24 usted by a computer staircase to determine a binocular Balance Point at which the observer reports th
26 +/- 0.21 logMAR to 0.27 +/- 0.25 logMAR and binocular BCVA improved from 0.33 +/- 0.2 logMAR to 0.17
27 nal prospective study 63 patients undergoing binocular cataract surgery were divided into four groups
28 electively desensitizing one or other of the binocular channels using untilted or non-oriented binocu
29 cells converged from both eyes, revealing a binocular combination mode in which functionally special
31 ocular energy model-a well-known model of V1 binocular complex cells-fails to signal disparity here.
32 out optical correction, and in monocular and binocular conditions; one condition was measured twice t
35 : habitual binocular distance visual acuity, binocular contrast sensitivity (CS), and the binocular d
38 AE remained "lazy" in high frequency domain, binocular contrast summation, and interocular phase comb
39 organization of topographic maps or disrupt binocular convergence in the superior colliculus.SIGNIFI
41 is believed to depend on the computation of binocular correlation by neurons in primary visual corte
42 e depth in a class of stimuli where the mean binocular correlation is 0 (half-matched random dot ster
43 ns can perceive depth in stimuli with a mean binocular correlation of zero (where a correlation-based
44 Strengthening of remaining eye inputs in the binocular cortex is followed by cross-modal adaptations
47 nimodal responses of the adult monocular and binocular cortices also mirror regional specificity in i
48 f the adaptation of the medial monocular and binocular cortices to long-term ME or dark exposure or a
51 pposite 3D directions generated by different binocular cues resulted in simultaneous, superimposed, o
53 und that monocular deprivation (MD), but not binocular deprivation (BD), increased dendritic spine el
56 this theory has been limited [3, 4], and the binocular differencing channel is missing from many mode
57 st common type of strabismus associated with binocular diplopia due to glaucoma surgery was hypertrop
60 The prevalence of monocular diplopia and binocular diplopia unrelated to glaucoma surgery was sim
61 ferences between the images in our two eyes, binocular disparities, to generate depth perception?
64 ese results also hold in models that include binocular disparity computations, providing a platform f
67 primates, MD also disrupts the emergence of binocular disparity selectivity, a cue resulting from in
68 nly on relative judgments of depth (relative binocular disparity) between objects, rather than judgme
69 ng for 4 cues (shading, motion, texture, and binocular disparity) with corresponding 2D and elementar
71 in macaque: (1) color versus luminance, (2) binocular disparity, (3) luminance contrast sensitivity,
72 ted largely by relative rather than absolute binocular disparity, and depth is perceived primarily fo
74 the better (P = .62) or worse (P = .88) eye, binocular distance (P = .15) or near (P = .23) visual ac
76 s of visual function were measured: habitual binocular distance visual acuity, binocular contrast sen
77 binocular contrast sensitivity (CS), and the binocular driving visual field constructed from combinin
78 correlated and anticorrelated dots, and the binocular energy model-a well-known model of V1 binocula
79 w that a straightforward modification to the binocular energy model-adding a point output nonlinearit
80 t of visual acuity is also fully reversed by binocular experience following treatment and, further, t
81 onfounding factors, each 1 dB/year change in binocular FDT mean sensitivity corresponded to a change
83 All participants underwent visual acuity, binocular function, visual field, optical coherence tomo
84 ments from baseline were significant for the binocular game (mean [SD] improvement, 1.5 [0.8] lines;
85 ed from baseline at the 2-week visit for the binocular game (mean [SD], 4.82 [2.82] vs 3.24 [2.87]; P
88 e 176 participants (22.2%) randomized to the binocular game and with log file data available performe
89 t in amblyopic eye BCVA was greater with the binocular game compared with patching, with a mean (SD)
90 han 13 years, amblyopic-eye VA improved with binocular game play and with patching, particularly in y
91 enrolled in the study, with 14 randomized to binocular game treatment and 14 to patching treatment.
93 logMAR (mean [SD], 1.7 [1.0] lines) for the binocular game vs a mean (SD) improvement of 0.16 (0.12)
94 hange, with children who crossed over to the binocular games catching up with children treated with b
97 games catching up with children treated with binocular games, for a mean (SD) improvement of 0.17 (0.
98 nes (2-sided 95% CI, 0.85-1.24 lines) in the binocular group and 1.35 lines (2-sided 95% CI, 1.17-1.5
99 ved by a mean (SD) of 2.5 (1.5) lines in the binocular group and 2.8 (0.8) lines in the patching grou
100 dence interval [CI]: 1.3-5.7 letters) in the binocular group and by 6.5 letters (2-sided 95% CI: 4.4-
101 escribed for 1 hour a day (190 participants; binocular group) or patching of the fellow eye prescribe
103 a type of neural network trained on natural binocular images can learn parameters that match key pro
107 ngs suggest that spatial-frequency dependent binocular imbalance may be useful for diagnosing amblyop
110 assification after routine examination using binocular indirect ophthalmoscopy (BIO) and obtained wid
111 ated tuberculosis, and point-of-care dilated binocular indirect ophthalmoscopy eye examination can pr
112 stem uses both velocity- and disparity-based binocular information for computing 3D motion, it is unk
114 Similarly, the proportion of patients with binocular inhibition (BiS score worse by at least 5 lett
118 ng the developmental critical period impairs binocular integration in mouse primary visual cortex.
119 Recent electrophysiological studies tested binocular integration in MT and found surprisingly that
121 c plaids, that opponent suppression precedes binocular integration, and that opponent suppression wil
122 Using microelectrode arrays, we examined binocular interaction in primary visual cortex and V2 of
125 we describe experiments in which we studied binocular interactions in macaques with experimentally i
127 Objectives: To assess the effectiveness of a binocular iPad (Apple Inc) adventure game as amblyopia t
129 re randomly assigned to either 16 weeks of a binocular iPad game prescribed for 1 hour a day (190 par
130 signed to treatment for 16 weeks of either a binocular iPad game prescribed for 1 hour per day (n = 4
131 rs, improvement in amblyopic eye VA with the binocular iPad game used in this study was not found to
135 ted that VA improvement with this particular binocular iPad treatment was not as good as with 2 hours
137 twork in simple analytical form and derive a binocular likelihood model that provides a unified accou
142 neurons determining a specific impairment of binocular matching of orientation preference, but leavin
143 ere, in mice of both sexes, we show that the binocular matching process is completely blocked by mono
144 veal ocular dominance as a key driver of the binocular matching process, and suggest a model whereby
145 gnitive stimulation, is sufficient to rescue binocular matching to the level seen in unmanipulated mi
147 nvestigate the association between change in binocular mean sensitivities and change in NEI VFQ-25 Ra
148 ly significant correlation between change in binocular mean sensitivity for FDT and change in NEI VFQ
153 hese results suggest that monocular, and not binocular, mechanisms set the limit of spatial acuity in
154 supports accumulating evidence that clinical binocular misalignment patterns are not reliable indicat
156 ion, each 0.5-dB/year faster rate of loss of binocular MS during follow-up was associated with a more
157 multivariate model, each 1-dB lower baseline binocular MS was associated with 34% higher odds of disa
158 visual field loss as estimated by integrated binocular MS were almost 4 times faster for those in who
159 ntoparallel motion studies concurrently in a binocular MT motion model, we generated clear, testable
160 a >/=3.0 to </=6.0 D associated with reduced binocular near VA (20/40 or worse) or reduced near stere
161 res were observed in hyperopic children with binocular near VA of 20/40 or worse (-8.5, P = 0.002 for
163 ses a shift in the response of visual cortex binocular neurons in favor of the nondeprived eye, a pro
165 s there was a statistical significant better binocular outcome in all distances (UDVA p = 0.036; UIVA
169 ds a new twist to the traditional view about binocular processing in the primate visual system and ra
172 r, intermediate, and distance visual acuity, binocular reading speed [International Reading Speed Tex
178 cture and function of connections underlying binocular responses of neurons in the visual cortex.
182 ate a robust, replicated autistic deficit in binocular rivalry [11], a basic visual function that is
183 tive advantage during a subsequent period of binocular rivalry [6-8], the robust form of visual compe
184 g behavioral and EEG results have shown that binocular rivalry and attention are intertwined: binocul
185 ed two different kinds of ambiguous stimuli, binocular rivalry and the phenomenon of ambiguous struct
187 ngs highlight the interplay of attention and binocular rivalry at multiple visual processing stages a
188 on, reveal that stimulus predominance during binocular rivalry can be realized both through an eye-sp
189 cular rivalry and attention are intertwined: binocular rivalry ceases when attention is diverted away
190 copy, we demonstrate a tight linkage between binocular rivalry dynamics in typical participants and b
191 ally, we show that the link between GABA and binocular rivalry dynamics is completely and specificall
192 he final result was a dramatic alteration in binocular rivalry dynamics, leading to profound predomin
194 dynamically under constant external inputs, binocular rivalry has been used for studying intrinsic c
195 found near stereoacuity and pAE dominance in binocular rivalry in "treated" amblyopia were largely co
197 rivalry, including the three hallmarks: (i) binocular rivalry requires attention; (ii) various perce
198 perceptual changes observed while viewing a binocular rivalry stimulus or an ambiguous structure-fro
199 demonstrate that the relative resistance of binocular rivalry to selective modulations gradually ero
200 lar suppression and related phenomena (e.g., binocular rivalry, flash suppression, continuous flash s
201 ion of the current computational theories of binocular rivalry, in which the role of attention is ign
202 However, existing studies have found that binocular rivalry-a phenomenon characterized by perceptu
209 comorbidity index, each 1 dB/year change in binocular SAP MS was associated with a change of 3.0 uni
213 ich the brain extracts 3D motion given these binocular signals, it is essential to understand how-or
218 ages presented to the two eyes such that the binocular summation and difference signals were tilted i
219 compelling evidence that the brain contains binocular summation and differencing channels that adapt
220 tly in the brain using mutually decorrelated binocular summation and differencing channels; when a ch
222 ese findings demonstrate that stereopsis and binocular summation are significantly correlated in pati
223 than those with better-eye, average-eye, and binocular summation of VA (r = -0.12 to -0.25), but not
224 , average eye, better or worse location, and binocular summation or pointwise binocular summation.
227 ormalization mechanisms resulting from early binocular suppression can explain much of these contrast
228 gularities were correlated with the level of binocular suppression in these V2 neurons and with the s
229 e noisy spiking is linked to a high level of binocular suppression in visual cortex during developmen
230 blyopic eye (AE) revealed a form of balanced binocular suppression that is unaltered in amblyopia.
234 ual Function Questionnaire [NEI VFQ-25]) and binocular tests or binocular approximations of visual fu
235 esults suggest that structured monocular and binocular training are necessary to fully recover defici
237 ar whether the minimal treatment response to binocular treatment was owing to poor treatment adherenc
238 game as amblyopia treatment and compare this binocular treatment with patching, the current standard
239 S): Three-months-postoperative monocular and binocular UCVA and DCVA in 4 m, 80 cm, and 40 cm (logMAR
241 linder -0.34 D +/- 0.38; FineVision Micro F, binocular UDVA, 0.01 logMAR +/- 0.05; monocular CDVA, 0.
242 ly, 93% of patients achieved 20/20 or better binocular UDVA; 90% and 97% of patients had J2 or better
243 0.06; binocular UNVA, 0.05 logMAR +/- 0.08; binocular UIVA, -0.05 logMAR +/- 0.12; spherical equival
244 included manifest refraction; monocular and binocular uncorrected (UCVA) and distance-corrected (DCV
245 an (+/- standard deviation) acuity: AT Lisa, binocular uncorrected distance visual acuity (UDVA), -0.
246 r visual acuity (DCNVA) of nondominant eyes; binocular uncorrected distance visual acuity (UDVA); unc
247 cuity (UNVA) at 40 cm, 0.05 logMAR +/- 0.08; binocular uncorrected intermediate visual acuity (UIVA)
248 visual acuity (CDVA), 0.02 logMAR +/- 0.06; binocular uncorrected near visual acuity (UNVA) at 40 cm
249 hieved in mesopic and photopic conditions in binocular uncorrected visual acuity and contrast sensiti
251 went: monocular defocus curve; monocular and binocular uncorrected visual acuity in photopic and meso
254 0.05; monocular CDVA, 0.03 logMAR +/- 0.06; binocular UNVA, 0.05 logMAR +/- 0.08; binocular UIVA, -0
255 e affects GABAergic synaptic transmission in binocular V1 and the plastic immunity observed at P45 is
257 n contrast, correlations between the VAQ and binocular VA (r = -0.07 to -0.21) were weaker in all sub
259 tests of visual function (Esterman VF score, binocular VA) were added to the CIGTS protocol 3 years i
261 f those patients exhibiting relatively early binocular VF loss had a defect confined to the inferior
262 for interpreting findings about location of binocular VF loss impacting everyday activities and exam
263 asurable VF loss in both eyes, superior-only binocular VF loss is more common than inferior-only loss
265 ontrast, we found some benefit in performing binocular VF testing, because the results correlated mor
266 n by the stronger eye often dominates during binocular viewing, blocking the image of the weaker eye
267 5 (4.5%) had at least 1 reported disorder of binocular vision (strabismus, 2.3%; diplopia, 2.2%; ambl
268 e two eyes on cortical mechanisms underlying binocular vision [1, 2], and experience's impact on this
270 OR) for the association between disorders of binocular vision and any of the 3 injury types was 2.23
272 toperative outcomes indicate improvements in binocular vision at far, intermediate, and near distance
273 in the visual cortex that reduce acuity and binocular vision by causing neurons to lose responsivene
274 near visual symptoms in children with normal binocular vision compared with symptoms caused by prefer
277 cent study provides compelling evidence that binocular vision uses two separate channels; one channel
285 responsiveness caused by MD is reversed when binocular visual experience follows temporary anesthetic
297 ts and binocular approximations to represent binocular visual function were assessed with Pearson's c
300 e mouse has emerged as a prominent model for binocular visual processing, yet little is known about t
WebLSDに未収録の専門用語(用法)は "新規対訳" から投稿できます。