戻る
「早戻しボタン」を押すと検索画面に戻ります。

今後説明を表示しない

[OK]

コーパス検索結果 (1語後でソート)

通し番号をクリックするとPubMedの該当ページを表示します
1 e in the degree to which neurons become more binocular.
2 inearly at two stages, one monocular and one binocular.
3                                 However, the binocular 10-2 sensitivities of 24-2 outliers had the st
4 sessed using a linear regression model, with binocular 10-2 VF sensitivity as the independent variabl
5 te NEI VFQ-25 score was associated with both binocular 24-2 (beta = 1.95; 95% CI, 0.47-3.43; P = .01)
6                                 Standardized binocular 24-2 and 10-2 VF sensitivities were calculated
7                               Association of binocular 24-2 and 10-2 VF sensitivity with Rasch-calibr
8  based on Cook distance of the regression of binocular 24-2 and NEI VFQ-25 score.
9 ular channels using untilted or non-oriented binocular adaptation patterns, we controlled the perceiv
10 tients in whom surgery successfully restored binocular alignment.
11 almost as gracefully as subjects with normal binocular alignment.
12 consistent with a sensory difference between binocular and dichoptic sensory mechanisms.
13 ased disparity tuning was most pronounced in binocular and ipsilaterally biased neurons, which are th
14  for baseline VA, the difference between the binocular and patching groups was -2.7 letters (95% CI:
15                                Importance: A binocular approach to treating anisometropic and strabis
16                                          The binocular approximations of binocular visual function we
17  score was correlated significantly with all binocular approximations of VF, with r values ranging fr
18 onnaire [NEI VFQ-25]) and binocular tests or binocular approximations of visual function.
19     Associations between binocular tests and binocular approximations to represent binocular visual f
20 ship of binocular visual function tests with binocular approximations using data from the Collaborati
21 nocular VA showed stronger correlations with binocular approximations, with r values ranging from 0.6
22 < 0.05 for all) than correlations with all 7 binocular approximations.
23  more closely with reported functioning than binocular approximations.
24 usted by a computer staircase to determine a binocular Balance Point at which the observer reports th
25 s syndrome and myopia improved monocular and binocular BCVA and contrast sensitivity.
26  +/- 0.21 logMAR to 0.27 +/- 0.25 logMAR and binocular BCVA improved from 0.33 +/- 0.2 logMAR to 0.17
27 nal prospective study 63 patients undergoing binocular cataract surgery were divided into four groups
28 electively desensitizing one or other of the binocular channels using untilted or non-oriented binocu
29  cells converged from both eyes, revealing a binocular combination mode in which functionally special
30 ly, we showed that, in amblyopic cortex, the binocular combination of signals is altered.
31 ocular energy model-a well-known model of V1 binocular complex cells-fails to signal disparity here.
32 out optical correction, and in monocular and binocular conditions; one condition was measured twice t
33                               In the clinic, binocular contrast sensitivity (CS) and better-eye visua
34                 At 3 months defocus testing, binocular contrast sensitivity (CS) under photopic and m
35 : habitual binocular distance visual acuity, binocular contrast sensitivity (CS), and the binocular d
36 diate (60 cm) and near (33 cm) distances and binocular contrast sensitivity.
37                                          The binocular contrast summation ratio is significantly lowe
38 AE remained "lazy" in high frequency domain, binocular contrast summation, and interocular phase comb
39  organization of topographic maps or disrupt binocular convergence in the superior colliculus.SIGNIFI
40 evealing increased eye movements and altered binocular coordination compared to head-fixed mice.
41  is believed to depend on the computation of binocular correlation by neurons in primary visual corte
42 e depth in a class of stimuli where the mean binocular correlation is 0 (half-matched random dot ster
43 ns can perceive depth in stimuli with a mean binocular correlation of zero (where a correlation-based
44 Strengthening of remaining eye inputs in the binocular cortex is followed by cross-modal adaptations
45 elopmental window resulting in maturation of binocular cortical cells and depth perception.
46 ng contrast in the AE decreased responses at binocular cortical sites.
47 nimodal responses of the adult monocular and binocular cortices also mirror regional specificity in i
48 f the adaptation of the medial monocular and binocular cortices to long-term ME or dark exposure or a
49            Our findings suggest that the two binocular cues for 3D motion might be processed by separ
50 ial to understand how-or indeed if-these two binocular cues interact.
51 pposite 3D directions generated by different binocular cues resulted in simultaneous, superimposed, o
52                                              Binocular defocus curve showed peaks with best visual ac
53 und that monocular deprivation (MD), but not binocular deprivation (BD), increased dendritic spine el
54 either pairing differ after monocular versus binocular deprivation [8-11].
55 ctile simultaneity after either monocular or binocular deprivation.
56 this theory has been limited [3, 4], and the binocular differencing channel is missing from many mode
57 st common type of strabismus associated with binocular diplopia due to glaucoma surgery was hypertrop
58                                              Binocular diplopia due to the glaucoma procedure was pre
59                                              Binocular diplopia not due to surgery was found in simil
60     The prevalence of monocular diplopia and binocular diplopia unrelated to glaucoma surgery was sim
61 ferences between the images in our two eyes, binocular disparities, to generate depth perception?
62 o investigate the effects of two depth cues, binocular disparity and relative size.
63        We find that, in humans, color versus binocular disparity columns extend one full area further
64 ese results also hold in models that include binocular disparity computations, providing a platform f
65                                        Using binocular disparity estimation as a concrete test case,
66                                              Binocular disparity is a powerful depth cue for object p
67  primates, MD also disrupts the emergence of binocular disparity selectivity, a cue resulting from in
68 nly on relative judgments of depth (relative binocular disparity) between objects, rather than judgme
69 ng for 4 cues (shading, motion, texture, and binocular disparity) with corresponding 2D and elementar
70 ever, generally share preferences for depth (binocular disparity).
71  in macaque: (1) color versus luminance, (2) binocular disparity, (3) luminance contrast sensitivity,
72 ted largely by relative rather than absolute binocular disparity, and depth is perceived primarily fo
73 ectively to stimulus variations in color and binocular disparity, respectively.
74 the better (P = .62) or worse (P = .88) eye, binocular distance (P = .15) or near (P = .23) visual ac
75 l performance, with no significant change in binocular distance vision or CS.
76 s of visual function were measured: habitual binocular distance visual acuity, binocular contrast sen
77 binocular contrast sensitivity (CS), and the binocular driving visual field constructed from combinin
78  correlated and anticorrelated dots, and the binocular energy model-a well-known model of V1 binocula
79 w that a straightforward modification to the binocular energy model-adding a point output nonlinearit
80 t of visual acuity is also fully reversed by binocular experience following treatment and, further, t
81 onfounding factors, each 1 dB/year change in binocular FDT mean sensitivity corresponded to a change
82                                   Integrated binocular fields were estimated from the monocular field
83    All participants underwent visual acuity, binocular function, visual field, optical coherence tomo
84 ments from baseline were significant for the binocular game (mean [SD] improvement, 1.5 [0.8] lines;
85 ed from baseline at the 2-week visit for the binocular game (mean [SD], 4.82 [2.82] vs 3.24 [2.87]; P
86 20/125]), with 14 children randomized to the binocular game and 14 to patching for 2 weeks.
87                               Interventions: Binocular game and patching as amblyopia treatments.
88 e 176 participants (22.2%) randomized to the binocular game and with log file data available performe
89 t in amblyopic eye BCVA was greater with the binocular game compared with patching, with a mean (SD)
90 han 13 years, amblyopic-eye VA improved with binocular game play and with patching, particularly in y
91 enrolled in the study, with 14 randomized to binocular game treatment and 14 to patching treatment.
92            Patching children crossed over to binocular game treatment, and all 28 children played the
93  logMAR (mean [SD], 1.7 [1.0] lines) for the binocular game vs a mean (SD) improvement of 0.16 (0.12)
94 hange, with children who crossed over to the binocular games catching up with children treated with b
95                Amblyopia can be treated with binocular games that rebalance contrast between the eyes
96                                              Binocular games that rebalance contrast to overcome supp
97 games catching up with children treated with binocular games, for a mean (SD) improvement of 0.17 (0.
98 nes (2-sided 95% CI, 0.85-1.24 lines) in the binocular group and 1.35 lines (2-sided 95% CI, 1.17-1.5
99 ved by a mean (SD) of 2.5 (1.5) lines in the binocular group and 2.8 (0.8) lines in the patching grou
100 dence interval [CI]: 1.3-5.7 letters) in the binocular group and by 6.5 letters (2-sided 95% CI: 4.4-
101 escribed for 1 hour a day (190 participants; binocular group) or patching of the fellow eye prescribe
102                                       In the binocular group, treatment adherence data from the iPad
103  a type of neural network trained on natural binocular images can learn parameters that match key pro
104                                              Binocular imaging with additional video cameras enabled
105                  Amblyopes showed pronounced binocular imbalance across all spatial frequencies, with
106                While amblyopia involves both binocular imbalance and deficits in processing high spat
107 ngs suggest that spatial-frequency dependent binocular imbalance may be useful for diagnosing amblyop
108 wn about the spatial-frequency dependence of binocular imbalance.
109 to the preoperative status, especially after binocular implantation.
110 assification after routine examination using binocular indirect ophthalmoscopy (BIO) and obtained wid
111 ated tuberculosis, and point-of-care dilated binocular indirect ophthalmoscopy eye examination can pr
112 stem uses both velocity- and disparity-based binocular information for computing 3D motion, it is unk
113            All participants were tested with binocular infrared video goggles with built-in laser tar
114   Similarly, the proportion of patients with binocular inhibition (BiS score worse by at least 5 lett
115                             Therefore, early binocular input is necessary to develop normal neural su
116                           While we find that binocular inputs to a subset of cells are important for
117 ncing channel is missing from many models of binocular integration [5-10].
118 ng the developmental critical period impairs binocular integration in mouse primary visual cortex.
119   Recent electrophysiological studies tested binocular integration in MT and found surprisingly that
120 els of this area have largely overlooked the binocular integration of motion signals.
121 c plaids, that opponent suppression precedes binocular integration, and that opponent suppression wil
122     Using microelectrode arrays, we examined binocular interaction in primary visual cortex and V2 of
123 sorder that alters both monocular vision and binocular interaction.
124                       To measure suppressive binocular interactions directly, we recorded neuronal re
125  we describe experiments in which we studied binocular interactions in macaques with experimentally i
126                                  We measured binocular interactions in visual cortex of anesthetized
127 Objectives: To assess the effectiveness of a binocular iPad (Apple Inc) adventure game as amblyopia t
128                               Interventions: Binocular iPad game or patching of the fellow eye.
129 re randomly assigned to either 16 weeks of a binocular iPad game prescribed for 1 hour a day (190 par
130 signed to treatment for 16 weeks of either a binocular iPad game prescribed for 1 hour per day (n = 4
131 rs, improvement in amblyopic eye VA with the binocular iPad game used in this study was not found to
132 t in teenagers with amblyopia treated with a binocular iPad game vs part-time patching.
133 nt in children with amblyopia treated with a binocular iPad game vs part-time patching.
134                 Conclusions and Relevance: A binocular iPad game was effective in treating childhood
135 ted that VA improvement with this particular binocular iPad treatment was not as good as with 2 hours
136         In contrast, visual deprivation with binocular lid suturing resulted in increased visual homo
137 twork in simple analytical form and derive a binocular likelihood model that provides a unified accou
138                                         Mean binocular logMAR near VA was better in emmetropic than i
139  forebrain excitatory neurons replicates the binocular matching deficits.
140                           Here, we show that binocular matching is completely blocked by monocular de
141                                              Binocular matching is thus mediated by orientation-speci
142 neurons determining a specific impairment of binocular matching of orientation preference, but leavin
143 ere, in mice of both sexes, we show that the binocular matching process is completely blocked by mono
144 veal ocular dominance as a key driver of the binocular matching process, and suggest a model whereby
145 gnitive stimulation, is sufficient to rescue binocular matching to the level seen in unmanipulated mi
146 individual layer 4 neurons and studied their binocular matching.
147 nvestigate the association between change in binocular mean sensitivities and change in NEI VFQ-25 Ra
148 ly significant correlation between change in binocular mean sensitivity for FDT and change in NEI VFQ
149           Each 1 decibel (dB)/year change in binocular mean sensitivity of the central inferior area
150 ls were used to calculate rates of change in binocular mean sensitivity over time.
151                   The mean rate of change in binocular mean sensitivity was faster for patients who r
152                                              Binocular mechanisms for visual processing are thought t
153 hese results suggest that monocular, and not binocular, mechanisms set the limit of spatial acuity in
154 supports accumulating evidence that clinical binocular misalignment patterns are not reliable indicat
155                                     We built binocular models in which signals from left- and right-e
156 ion, each 0.5-dB/year faster rate of loss of binocular MS during follow-up was associated with a more
157 multivariate model, each 1-dB lower baseline binocular MS was associated with 34% higher odds of disa
158 visual field loss as estimated by integrated binocular MS were almost 4 times faster for those in who
159 ntoparallel motion studies concurrently in a binocular MT motion model, we generated clear, testable
160 a >/=3.0 to </=6.0 D associated with reduced binocular near VA (20/40 or worse) or reduced near stere
161 res were observed in hyperopic children with binocular near VA of 20/40 or worse (-8.5, P = 0.002 for
162                      Accommodative response, binocular near VA, and near stereoacuity were measured.
163 ses a shift in the response of visual cortex binocular neurons in favor of the nondeprived eye, a pro
164                                              Binocular neurons that are well matched in spatial frequ
165 s there was a statistical significant better binocular outcome in all distances (UDVA p = 0.036; UIVA
166  panoramic visual field with a small frontal binocular overlap of 20 degrees .
167 ess effective as the same contrast in pFE in binocular phase combination.
168 l processing of eye-specific visual input in binocular primary visual cortex.
169 ds a new twist to the traditional view about binocular processing in the primate visual system and ra
170 f dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus in early binocular processing.
171                       The URE was defined as binocular PVA of </= 20/30, improving to >20/30 with sub
172 r, intermediate, and distance visual acuity, binocular reading speed [International Reading Speed Tex
173 tion persisted during subsequent 2-4 days of binocular recovery.
174 r BD tended to be reversed during subsequent binocular recovery.
175 ndritic spine elimination over 3 days in the binocular region of 4-week-old adolescent mice.
176 oviding a platform for future exploration of binocular response properties in MT.
177  known about the spatial frequency tuning of binocular responses in mouse visual cortex.
178 cture and function of connections underlying binocular responses of neurons in the visual cortex.
179                                              Binocular responses were obtained with the head and trun
180       Comparing the monocular results to the binocular results there was a statistical significant be
181          Whereas 10 days of dark exposure or binocular retinal inactivation were not better at promot
182 ate a robust, replicated autistic deficit in binocular rivalry [11], a basic visual function that is
183 tive advantage during a subsequent period of binocular rivalry [6-8], the robust form of visual compe
184 g behavioral and EEG results have shown that binocular rivalry and attention are intertwined: binocul
185 ed two different kinds of ambiguous stimuli, binocular rivalry and the phenomenon of ambiguous struct
186                Although VA, stereoacuity and binocular rivalry at low spatial frequency in treated am
187 ngs highlight the interplay of attention and binocular rivalry at multiple visual processing stages a
188 on, reveal that stimulus predominance during binocular rivalry can be realized both through an eye-sp
189 cular rivalry and attention are intertwined: binocular rivalry ceases when attention is diverted away
190 copy, we demonstrate a tight linkage between binocular rivalry dynamics in typical participants and b
191 ally, we show that the link between GABA and binocular rivalry dynamics is completely and specificall
192 he final result was a dramatic alteration in binocular rivalry dynamics, leading to profound predomin
193                                       In two binocular rivalry experiments, we manipulated the sense
194  dynamically under constant external inputs, binocular rivalry has been used for studying intrinsic c
195 found near stereoacuity and pAE dominance in binocular rivalry in "treated" amblyopia were largely co
196                         This large change in binocular rivalry predominance was driven by two factors
197  rivalry, including the three hallmarks: (i) binocular rivalry requires attention; (ii) various perce
198  perceptual changes observed while viewing a binocular rivalry stimulus or an ambiguous structure-fro
199  demonstrate that the relative resistance of binocular rivalry to selective modulations gradually ero
200 lar suppression and related phenomena (e.g., binocular rivalry, flash suppression, continuous flash s
201 ion of the current computational theories of binocular rivalry, in which the role of attention is ign
202    However, existing studies have found that binocular rivalry-a phenomenon characterized by perceptu
203    Here, we provide a computational model of binocular rivalry.
204 nhibition: spatial suppression of motion and binocular rivalry.
205 he deprived eye perceptual boost measured by binocular rivalry.
206                                  Progressive binocular RNFL thickness loss was associated with worsen
207 cores was associated uniquely with change in binocular RNFL thickness.
208           Each 1 decibel (dB)/year change in binocular SAP MS was associated with a change of 2.0 uni
209  comorbidity index, each 1 dB/year change in binocular SAP MS was associated with a change of 3.0 uni
210 he GDS scores during follow-up and change in binocular SAP sensitivity.
211  Rasch scores during follow-up and change in binocular SAP sensitivity.
212                      We found that these two binocular signals are processed distinctly at the levels
213 ich the brain extracts 3D motion given these binocular signals, it is essential to understand how-or
214 of 10 prism diopters or less and evidence of binocular single vision).
215 encing channels that adapt to the prevailing binocular statistics.
216                                              Binocular stereopsis is one of the primary cues for thre
217 cuity, contrast sensitivity, vernier acuity, binocular stereopsis, and visual perception.
218 ages presented to the two eyes such that the binocular summation and difference signals were tilted i
219  compelling evidence that the brain contains binocular summation and differencing channels that adapt
220 tly in the brain using mutually decorrelated binocular summation and differencing channels; when a ch
221                     The relationship between binocular summation and stereoacuity was studied by Spea
222 ese findings demonstrate that stereopsis and binocular summation are significantly correlated in pati
223 than those with better-eye, average-eye, and binocular summation of VA (r = -0.12 to -0.25), but not
224 , average eye, better or worse location, and binocular summation or pointwise binocular summation.
225 cation, and binocular summation or pointwise binocular summation.
226 es ranging from 0.65 (worse-eye VA) to 0.80 (binocular summation; P < 0.0001 for all).
227 ormalization mechanisms resulting from early binocular suppression can explain much of these contrast
228 gularities were correlated with the level of binocular suppression in these V2 neurons and with the s
229 e noisy spiking is linked to a high level of binocular suppression in visual cortex during developmen
230 blyopic eye (AE) revealed a form of balanced binocular suppression that is unaltered in amblyopia.
231                  We found limited benefit in binocular testing of VA in the clinical setting as a mea
232                         Associations between binocular tests and binocular approximations to represen
233                                              Binocular tests of visual function (Esterman VF score, b
234 ual Function Questionnaire [NEI VFQ-25]) and binocular tests or binocular approximations of visual fu
235 esults suggest that structured monocular and binocular training are necessary to fully recover defici
236               However, it is unclear whether binocular treatment is comparable to patching in treatin
237 ar whether the minimal treatment response to binocular treatment was owing to poor treatment adherenc
238 game as amblyopia treatment and compare this binocular treatment with patching, the current standard
239 S): Three-months-postoperative monocular and binocular UCVA and DCVA in 4 m, 80 cm, and 40 cm (logMAR
240                                              Binocular UCVA at distance, intermediate, and near was -
241 linder -0.34 D +/- 0.38; FineVision Micro F, binocular UDVA, 0.01 logMAR +/- 0.05; monocular CDVA, 0.
242 ly, 93% of patients achieved 20/20 or better binocular UDVA; 90% and 97% of patients had J2 or better
243  0.06; binocular UNVA, 0.05 logMAR +/- 0.08; binocular UIVA, -0.05 logMAR +/- 0.12; spherical equival
244  included manifest refraction; monocular and binocular uncorrected (UCVA) and distance-corrected (DCV
245 an (+/- standard deviation) acuity: AT Lisa, binocular uncorrected distance visual acuity (UDVA), -0.
246 r visual acuity (DCNVA) of nondominant eyes; binocular uncorrected distance visual acuity (UDVA); unc
247 cuity (UNVA) at 40 cm, 0.05 logMAR +/- 0.08; binocular uncorrected intermediate visual acuity (UIVA)
248  visual acuity (CDVA), 0.02 logMAR +/- 0.06; binocular uncorrected near visual acuity (UNVA) at 40 cm
249 hieved in mesopic and photopic conditions in binocular uncorrected visual acuity and contrast sensiti
250                      All patients achieved a binocular uncorrected visual acuity better than 0.3 LogM
251 went: monocular defocus curve; monocular and binocular uncorrected visual acuity in photopic and meso
252                                         Mean binocular uncorrected visual acuity in photopic conditio
253                                      Mesopic binocular uncorrected visual acuity values were similar
254  0.05; monocular CDVA, 0.03 logMAR +/- 0.06; binocular UNVA, 0.05 logMAR +/- 0.08; binocular UIVA, -0
255 e affects GABAergic synaptic transmission in binocular V1 and the plastic immunity observed at P45 is
256  (VA) <20/32 and >20/100, while controls had binocular VA >20/32.
257 n contrast, correlations between the VAQ and binocular VA (r = -0.07 to -0.21) were weaker in all sub
258                                              Binocular VA showed stronger correlations with binocular
259 tests of visual function (Esterman VF score, binocular VA) were added to the CIGTS protocol 3 years i
260 dination of monocular saccade amplitudes and binocular vergence eye movements.
261 f those patients exhibiting relatively early binocular VF loss had a defect confined to the inferior
262  for interpreting findings about location of binocular VF loss impacting everyday activities and exam
263 asurable VF loss in both eyes, superior-only binocular VF loss is more common than inferior-only loss
264                           Common patterns of binocular VF loss were dominated chiefly by superior hem
265 ontrast, we found some benefit in performing binocular VF testing, because the results correlated mor
266 n by the stronger eye often dominates during binocular viewing, blocking the image of the weaker eye
267 5 (4.5%) had at least 1 reported disorder of binocular vision (strabismus, 2.3%; diplopia, 2.2%; ambl
268 e two eyes on cortical mechanisms underlying binocular vision [1, 2], and experience's impact on this
269  developing visual cortex can cause impaired binocular vision and amblyopia.
270 OR) for the association between disorders of binocular vision and any of the 3 injury types was 2.23
271                                 Disorders of binocular vision are increasingly prevalent among fee-fo
272 toperative outcomes indicate improvements in binocular vision at far, intermediate, and near distance
273  in the visual cortex that reduce acuity and binocular vision by causing neurons to lose responsivene
274 near visual symptoms in children with normal binocular vision compared with symptoms caused by prefer
275 ia and as an outcome measure for recovery of binocular vision following therapy.
276                                              Binocular vision in amblyopes is often disrupted by inte
277 cent study provides compelling evidence that binocular vision uses two separate channels; one channel
278                                Patients with binocular vision will be sensitive to diplopia in any ga
279 yes, likely a key step in the development of binocular vision.
280 in individuals with and without disorders of binocular vision.
281            Presenting distance uniocular and binocular visual acuity were assessed using a 3-m logMAR
282                                The mammalian binocular visual circuit is comprised of projections of
283         Furthermore, we find that neurons in binocular visual cortex that respond only to the contral
284                           Using monocular or binocular visual deprivation, we examined the effects of
285 responsiveness caused by MD is reversed when binocular visual experience follows temporary anesthetic
286  the mean sensitivity (MS) of the integrated binocular visual field (BVF).
287                 Association between rates of binocular visual field loss and self-reported number of
288         Participants who passed the Esterman binocular visual field test for driving in the United Ki
289                                An integrated binocular visual field was estimated from the monocular
290 , and peripheral superior) of the integrated binocular visual field.
291 sing mean sensitivity (MS) of the integrated binocular visual field.
292         Mean sensitivities of the integrated binocular visual fields were estimated for FDT and SAP a
293                                              Binocular visual function (VF and VA) and VR QOL.
294 and closure of critical period and deficient binocular visual function in mature animals.
295 d seventy-five patients underwent at least 1 binocular visual function test.
296                To assess the relationship of binocular visual function tests with binocular approxima
297 ts and binocular approximations to represent binocular visual function were assessed with Pearson's c
298              The binocular approximations of binocular visual function were better or worse eye, aver
299 en no systematic study on both monocular and binocular visual functions.
300 e mouse has emerged as a prominent model for binocular visual processing, yet little is known about t

WebLSDに未収録の専門用語(用法)は "新規対訳" から投稿できます。
 
Page Top