コーパス検索結果 (1語後でソート)
通し番号をクリックするとPubMedの該当ページを表示します
1 processes, which in turn leads to irrational choice behavior.
2 ferentially engaged in effort and cue-guided choice behavior.
3 oportions matching those that best explained choice behavior.
4 rgets, ensuring that total reward was due to choice behavior.
5 ce of conceptual knowledge and its effect on choice behavior.
6 3 receptor agonist, 7-OH-DPAT, did not alter choice behavior.
7 effort can be used to assess effort-related choice behavior.
8 perience to the contrary, instructions drove choice behavior.
9 hat value, risk, and risk aversion influence choice behavior.
10 e influencing RTs and errors, did not affect choice behavior.
11 ing an overall metric of value used to guide choice behavior.
12 aptic plasticity is able to produce adaptive choice behavior.
13 choice outcome, novelty nevertheless drives choice behavior.
14 he two sets of brain regions predicts actual choice behavior.
15 the evolution of qualitative aspects of its choice behavior.
16 e-exposed rats displayed increased impulsive choice behavior.
17 experience and can exert strong influence on choice behavior.
18 einforcement learning model of the patient's choice behavior.
19 ed individual differences in satiety-related choice behavior.
20 ositive and negative outcomes to guide their choice behavior.
21 ter self-control and improved predictions of choice behavior.
22 ortance of the law of diminishing returns in choice behavior.
23 que to modulate dopamine activity and monkey choice behavior.
24 romedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) predicted choice behavior.
25 not use prospective regret signals to guide choice behavior.
26 volved in Pavlovian processes that influence choice behavior.
27 phasic dopamine release that may drive risky choice behavior.
28 formation and use of prior beliefs to guide choice behavior.
29 avioral mechanism by which MA rigidly biases choice behavior.
30 ous reward, and are predictive of subsequent choice behavior.
31 animals integrate prior knowledge into their choice behavior.
32 ognized as a ubiquitous aspect of real-world choice behavior.
33 significant component of the variability of choice behavior.
34 to motor actions, thereby enabling adaptive choice behavior.
35 question of causal factors underlying human choice behavior.
36 site ways with the baseline stochasticity of choice behavior.
37 ssion-like effects, can alter effort-related choice behavior.
38 racterize our model of individual and social choice behavior.
39 rcuits govern the ability to learn and shift choice behavior.
40 pulations of value are sufficient to mediate choice behavior.
41 ge to future actions is crucial for adaptive choice behavior.
42 e neural mechanisms mediating this important choice behavior.
43 and result in more impulsive social-economic choice behavior.
44 ulsivity, whereas valproate had no effect on choice behavior.
45 ompeting valuation systems in the control of choice behavior.
46 ained monkeys can serve as a model for human choice behavior.
47 the inability of rats to maintain reinforced choice behavior.
48 yses to identify regions involved in biasing choice behavior.
49 renicline on probabilistic reversal learning choice behavior.
50 e of the selection pressures associated with choice behaviors.
52 issors game, rhesus monkeys can adjust their choice behaviors according to both actual and hypothetic
54 commissurotomy, were unable to adjust their choice behavior after a change in the outcome (here, a r
55 thyltransferase gene predicts both impulsive choice behavior and activity levels in the dPFC and PPC
56 l substrates of such mechanisms by comparing choice behavior and blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD)
58 oblem was assessed by monitoring accuracy of choice behavior and by measuring latency to respond for
59 of purely economic motivations in explaining choice behavior and instead emphasize the importance of
61 alue coding critically influences stochastic choice behavior and provide a generalizable quantitative
62 estigate amino acid-specific effects on food-choice behavior and report that folic acid from the micr
63 provides evidence for OFC's role in guiding choice behavior and shows that this is dissociable from
64 key role of orbitofrontal cortex activity in choice behavior and shows that this is dissociable from
65 s were screened for aggressive and impulsive choice behaviors and categorized into Low-Aggression (L-
67 nstrates that the two dominant frameworks of choice behavior are linked through the law of diminishin
70 tion and explains both classically described choice behavior as well as behavioral patterns not predi
71 ng how marketing actions can affect consumer choice behavior as well as for how environmental cues ca
73 ce that enhanced cognitive control can shift choice behavior away from immediate and risky rewards, w
75 e has an impact on monkeys' ability to guide choice behavior based on reward value but does not impac
76 magination can be used to accurately predict choice behavior both between and within individuals.
79 re can cause enduring increases in impulsive choice behavior, consistent with observations in human s
83 uggested that tasks measuring effort-related choice behavior could be used as animal models of the mo
85 trials and reinforcer intake, but effects on choice behavior did not depend on these motivational cha
86 ve payoffs and primary rewards, the animal's choice behavior during this task was nearly optimal.
90 Here, we combined computational modeling of choice behavior, experimentally induced inflammation, an
91 udy the neurophysiological signals governing choice behavior fall under one of two major theoretical
92 a key prediction of this hypothesis, we fit choice behavior from a dynamic foraging task with reinfo
96 lude that previous exposure to cocaine makes choice behavior hypersensitive to differences in the tim
97 also the case in rodents, we examined rat's choice behavior in a binary choice task in which variabl
98 hat treatment with dopaminergic drugs alters choice behavior in a manner consistent with the theory.
99 nhibition of the BLA has opposite effects on choice behavior in a rat model of risky decision making,
104 igate this function, we studied instrumental choice behavior in mice lacking GPR88, a striatum-enrich
107 t fixed but collapse over time, facilitating choice behavior in the presence of low-quality evidence.
112 s indicate that the influence of dopamine on choice behavior involves a specific modulation of the at
113 The ability to use prior knowledge to adapt choice behavior is critical for flexible decision making
114 ggesting that information relevant for risky choice behavior is encoded in coarse global patterns of
116 This neural pattern, as well as subjects' choice behavior, is consistent with a teaching signal fo
119 that emotional associations have on survival choice behaviors may lead to better treatments for menta
121 han gradually adjusting their time-dependent choice behavior, mice abruptly adopted temporal decision
126 video games for measures of brain activity, choice behavior, or cognitive performance.SIGNIFICANCE S
130 matching"-a consistent example of suboptimal choice behavior seen in humans-occurs in an optimal Baye
133 ovement initiation produced a robust bias in choice behavior, this bias was substantially diminished
135 retrieved during episodic sampling can cause choice behavior to deviate sharply from the predictions
138 ngle, unified decision process that mediates choice behavior via a common neural currency for outcome
141 design allowed us to test whether subjects' choice behavior was guided by policy-based methods, whic
143 plore the neural basis of such intertemporal choice behavior, we devised a novel two-alternative choi
144 s causally related to specific components of choice behavior, we employed selective optogenetic stimu
145 in D. melanogaster and mediate critical host-choice behavior, were deleted or pseudogenized in the ge
146 Notably, OFC inactivation did not affect choice behavior when it was guided by innate taste avers
147 The AcbC-lesioned rats produced appropriate choice behavior when the reward magnitude was equal.
148 Our results show that context-dependent choice behavior, which is commonly perceived as an irrat
149 ble to produce a bidirectional modulation of choice behavior, while drugs that act on D3 receptors we
150 tance and polarization-dependent female mate choice behavior with no polarization-dependent courtship
151 rative data indicate that context influences choice behavior, with the strongest effect seen in marmo
WebLSDに未収録の専門用語(用法)は "新規対訳" から投稿できます。