戻る
「早戻しボタン」を押すと検索画面に戻ります。

今後説明を表示しない

[OK]

コーパス検索結果 (1語後でソート)

通し番号をクリックするとPubMedの該当ページを表示します
1 posers to write and edit music while totally deaf.
2 ensuing adult transgenic mice are profoundly deaf.
3 ially improving hearing for children who are deaf.
4 nd hear speech in noise for children who are deaf.
5 iobp(Deltaex8/Deltaex8)) that are profoundly deaf.
6 no detectable ABR and by P30 these mice were deaf.
7 ced AF connectivity was observed in the tone deaf.
8 fness in humans, and OTOF knock-out mice are deaf.
9 lacking PCs owing to a mutation in Fgfr3 are deaf.
10 ecruited for visual and tactile input in the deaf.
11 lly deaf, and 7.5% (N = 15) were bilaterally deaf.
12 767Serfs*21 mutation (72.1%) were moderately deaf.
13 region of Tdrd1 containing the myeloid Nervy DEAF-1 (MYND) domain and the first two Tudor domains.
14 oteins with the N-terminal Myeloid Nervy and DEAF-1 (MYND)-type zinc finger of PHD2.
15                                              DEAF-1 activates the expression of Mtk and Drs promoter-
16 ification technology (MudPIT), we identified DEAF-1 as a candidate regulator.
17                                 A functional Deaf-1 binding site on the mouse 5-HT1A promoter was rec
18 X assays and footprinting data indicate that DEAF-1 binds to and activates Mtk and Drs regulatory DNA
19                               In cell models Deaf-1 displays dual activity, repressing 5-HT1A autorec
20               We have disrupted the Lmo4 and Deaf-1 genes in mice to define their biological function
21 imarily nuclear, cytoplasmic localization of DEAF-1 has been observed, and this suggests the presence
22  Deaf-1(-/-) mice indicate the importance of Deaf-1 in regulation of 5-HT1A gene expression and provi
23  the mouse 5-HT1A promoter was recognized by Deaf-1 in vitro and in vivo and mediated dual activity o
24                     To address regulation by Deaf-1 in vivo, Deaf-1 knock-out mice bred to a C57BL/6
25                              We propose that DEAF-1 is a regulator of Drosophila immunity.
26     To address regulation by Deaf-1 in vivo, Deaf-1 knock-out mice bred to a C57BL/6 background were
27                         In the dorsal raphe, Deaf-1 knock-out mice displayed increased 5-HT1A mRNA, p
28                               Thus, Lmo4 and Deaf-1 mutant mice exhibit overlapping as well as distin
29                                  Remarkably, Deaf-1 mutants displayed phenotypic abnormalities simila
30 st to Lmo4 nullizygous mice, nonexencephalic Deaf-1 mutants remained healthy.
31 ro and in vivo and mediated dual activity of Deaf-1 on 5-HT1A gene transcription.
32 rotein interaction assay identified a DEAF-1/DEAF-1 protein interaction domain within the NES region.
33 o eliminates DNA binding, demonstrating that DEAF-1 protein-protein interaction is required for DNA r
34 e Diptericin (Dpt) regulatory region confers DEAF-1 responsiveness to this normally DEAF-1-independen
35  and NLS provides a basis for the control of DEAF-1 subcellular localization and function, whereas th
36                          The coexpression of DEAF-1 with Dorsal, Dif, and Relish results in the syner
37 expression and reduced raphe 5-HT content in Deaf-1(-/-) mice indicate the importance of Deaf-1 in re
38  Arabidopsis SAND (Sp100, AIRE-1, NucP41/75, DEAF-1) domain protein ULTRAPETALA1 (ULT1) functions as
39  Deformed epidermal autoregulatory factor-1 (DEAF-1) is a DNA-binding protein required for embryonic
40  deformed epidermal autoregulatory factor-1 (DEAF-1), also contributes to the immune response and is
41 ZMYND8 (zinc finger MYND (Myeloid, Nervy and DEAF-1)-type containing 8), a newly identified component
42 nfers DEAF-1 responsiveness to this normally DEAF-1-independent enhancer.
43  cells were reduced in the frontal cortex of Deaf-1-null mice, with no significant change in hippocam
44 scent protein interaction assay identified a DEAF-1/DEAF-1 protein interaction domain within the NES
45                     The transcription factor Deaf-1/NUDR has been identified as one partner protein o
46 hism (rs6295), which prevents the binding of Deaf-1/NUDR leading to dysregulation of the receptor, ha
47 8 (zinc finger and MYND [myeloid, Nervy, and DEAF-1] domain containing 8) as a novel DDR factor that
48                    The study of congenitally deaf adult humans provides an opportunity to examine neu
49 ent using the Multiple Object Tracking task, Deaf adult native signers and hearing non-signers also p
50  first experiment using an enumeration task, Deaf adult native signers and hearing non-signers perfor
51                                              Deaf adults and children can be successfully (re)integra
52 s have restored hearing in more than 200 000 deaf adults and children to a level that allows most to
53 rtex to visual speech in the same profoundly deaf adults both before and 6 mo after implantation.
54 d the strength of the visual illusion in the deaf adults in line with the interpretation that the ill
55                                 In addition, deaf adults perceived bimodal stimuli differently; in co
56                          Here we enrolled 15 deaf and 15 hearing adults into an functional MRI experi
57 The zebrafish pinball wizard (pwi) mutant is deaf and blind.
58 19P) of TRPML3 (mucolipin 3), are profoundly deaf and display vestibular and pigmentation deficiencie
59 n in the left inferior frontal gyrus in both deaf and dyslexic adults when contrasted with hearing no
60  The data indicate greater activation in the deaf and dyslexic groups than in the hearing non-dyslexi
61 elated Cib2 mutation, and show that both are deaf and exhibit no mechanotransduction in auditory hair
62  lay the foundation for the clinical care of deaf and hard-of-hearing persons in the future.
63                                     They are deaf and have vestibular dysfunction but do not develop
64     Varitint-waddler (Va and Va(J)) mice are deaf and have vestibular impairment, with inner ear defe
65  gray (GMV) and white (WMV) matter volume in deaf and hearing native users of ASL, as well as deaf an
66  and hearing native users of ASL, as well as deaf and hearing native users of English.
67 l cortices (STC) we collected fMRI data from deaf and hearing participants (male and female), who eit
68 s with meaningless gestures by pre-lingually Deaf and hearing participants.
69                                         Both deaf and hearing signers exhibited an increased volume o
70                                         Both deaf and hearing subjects performed the task visually, i
71               We thus questioned whether the deaf and immature human auditory system is able to integ
72            In the absence of FGF20, mice are deaf and lateral compartment cells remain undifferentiat
73 urtship vocalizations emitted by chronically deaf and normal hearing adult male mice.
74  of mouse Kcnq1 models JLNS in that mice are deaf and show abnormal ECGs.
75 ted deletion of Tmc1 (Tmc1(Delta) mice) were deaf and those with a deletion of Tmc2 (Tmc2(Delta) mice
76 mechanotransduction, we discovered a line of deaf and uncoordinated zebrafish with defective hair-cel
77 also described for further investigating the deaf and vestibular mutants identified in the primary sc
78 al hearing, 18.1% (N = 36) were unilaterally deaf, and 7.5% (N = 15) were bilaterally deaf.
79 r principal neurons from the MSO of hearing, deaf, and deaf cats with cochlear implants.
80 any of their best compositions while totally deaf, and Georg Friedrich Handel and Frederick Delius st
81 humans, mice, and zebrafish; individuals are deaf, and stereocilia are disorganized.
82 erior visual motion detection ability in the deaf animal.
83 aphically organized, "isofrequency" bands in deaf animals over the ages examined, P18-P70.
84                                         Late-deaf animals showed small-scale changes in projections f
85 matic terminals was significantly smaller in deaf animals when compared with hearing animals.
86 A1 that differ between early- and late-onset deaf animals, suggesting that potential crossmodal activ
87 ures we examined were similar in hearing and deaf animals.
88  are congenitally hypothyroid and profoundly deaf as a consequence when the condition is untreated.
89 difference in STM span, hearing speakers and deaf ASL users have comparable working memory resources
90 both baringo and nice mutants are profoundly deaf at the age of 4 weeks.
91 ntified reliably in the vast majority of the deaf, at the single subject level, despite the absence o
92 nsive and auditory-responsive neurons in the deaf auditory cortex formed two distinct populations tha
93 tion are abolished when a specific region of deaf auditory cortex, the dorsal zone (DZ), is deactivat
94 altered cross-modal organization observed in deaf auditory cortex.
95 r electrical stimulation of the congenitally deaf auditory system via cochlear implants would restore
96 y heterogeneous group of autosomal recessive deaf-blinding disorders.
97  the motor protein myosin VIIA (MYO7A) cause deaf-blindness (Usher syndrome type 1B, USH1B) and nonsy
98                     CDH23 null alleles cause deaf-blindness (Usher syndrome type 1D; USH1D), whereas
99         Usher syndrome type 1B is a combined deaf-blindness condition caused by mutations in the MYO7
100          Mutations in the MYO7A gene cause a deaf-blindness disorder, known as Usher syndrome 1B.
101 er syndrome (USH) is the most common form of deaf-blindness in humans.
102  syndrome (USH) is the most common inherited deaf-blindness with the majority of USH causative genes
103    Mutations in PCDH15 cause Usher Syndrome (deaf-blindness) and recessive deafness.
104 fects, olfactory dysfunction, growth delays, deaf-blindness, balance disorders and congenital heart m
105  (USH) is the most common cause of inherited deaf-blindness, manifested as USH1, USH2 and USH3 clinic
106 rome (USH) is the leading cause of inherited deaf-blindness, with type 2 (USH2) being the most common
107 her syndrome is the leading cause of genetic deaf-blindness.
108 ead to non-syndromic deafness, blindness and deaf-blindness.
109 sher syndrome, the most common form of human deaf-blindness.
110         Usher syndrome is the major cause of deaf/blindness in the world.
111 tocadherin-15, a product of the gene for the deaf/blindness Usher syndrome type 1F/DFNB23 locus.
112     We asked two questions regarding how the deaf brain in humans adapts to sensory deprivation: (1)
113                 These Ush1 knockout mice are deaf but do not recapitulate vision defects before 10 mo
114 lants (CIs) partially restore hearing to the deaf by directly stimulating the inner ear.
115 estore hearing cues in the severe-profoundly deaf by electrically stimulating spiral ganglion neurons
116 revealed that S1P(2) receptor-null mice were deaf by one month of age.
117 cell defects by postnatal day 2 (P2) and are deaf by P21-P25.
118 e hearing loss at the age of 4 weeks and are deaf by the age of 8 weeks, whereas both baringo and nic
119 ements indicate that these mice are severely deaf by the third week of life.
120 rticipants.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Those born deaf can offer unique insights into neuroplasticity, in
121 mplete sensory deprivation, the congenitally deaf cat.
122 higher-order auditory fields in congenitally deaf cats (CDCs).
123 ical areas in adult hearing and congenitally deaf cats (CDCs): the primary auditory field A1, two sec
124 ity to low-rate pulse trains in congenitally deaf cats compared with acutely deafened cats.
125                   We stimulated congenitally deaf cats for 3 months with a six-channel cochlear impla
126           Three and 6-month-old congenitally deaf cats received unilateral cochlear implants and were
127 l neurons from the MSO of hearing, deaf, and deaf cats with cochlear implants.
128 ere significantly larger than the boutons in deaf cats, although not as large as in the hearing cats,
129 Additionally, in a subset of early- and late-deaf cats, area 17 and the lateral posterior nucleus (LP
130                                     In early-deaf cats, ipsilateral neuronal labeling in visual and s
131 best ITDs were more variable in congenitally deaf cats, leading to poorer ITD coding within the natur
132                                      In late-deaf cats, projections from the anterior auditory field
133                                           In deaf cats, substantially reduced induced responses were
134                       Specifically, in early-deaf cats, the anterior auditory field (AAF) is unrespon
135 und to be significantly larger than those of deaf cats.
136 ed in cross-modal (visual) reorganization in deaf cats.
137 ield (but not in primary auditory cortex) of deaf cats.
138 hlear implants in adult hearing controls and deaf cats.
139 elopment that were not pruned postnatally in deaf cats.
140 ith less marked differences observed in late-deaf cats.
141 ochlear implants provide sound perception to deaf children and can mitigate, to varying extents, the
142 ecome a standard clinical procedure for born-deaf children and its success has led over the years to
143                                  Research on deaf children and sign language acquisition can broaden
144 r example, after cochlear implantation, some deaf children develop spoken language skills approaching
145 early childhood cochlear-implant, profoundly deaf children do not develop intact, high-level, auditor
146  normal hearing children and in congenitally deaf children fitted with cochlear implants.
147 ncomplementary matings that can produce only deaf children has increased by a factor of more than fiv
148  limitations of implantation in congenitally deaf children.
149  concept formation are at particular risk in deaf children.
150 that account for as many as 2% of profoundly deaf children; however, the underlying cause for its dos
151                                   Retinas of deaf circler (dfcr) mice, which possess mutant Ush1c, we
152 circler' (allele symbol dfcr) and the other 'deaf circler 2 Jackson' (allele symbol dfcr-2J).
153                               One was named 'deaf circler' (allele symbol dfcr) and the other 'deaf c
154                               Moreover, the 'deaf-circler' dfcr mutant form of harmonin, which does n
155 ation of nonsensory cells that remain in the deaf cochlea.
156                       Sign languages used by deaf communities around the world possess the same struc
157 disorders occur at rates 2-3 times higher in deaf compared with hearing children.
158 icant differences between normal-hearing and deaf controls.
159 sensitivity and prevented hair cell death in deaf Cx30-/- mice.
160 t of binaural processing in children who are deaf despite early access to bilateral auditory input by
161 mate transporter-3 (VGLUT3) are congenitally deaf due to loss of glutamate release at the inner hair
162 have a normal endocochlear potential but are deaf due to rapid degeneration of cochlear outer hair ce
163 port that mice lacking VGLUT3 are profoundly deaf due to the absence of glutamate release from hair c
164 ccess with which electrical stimulation of a deaf ear can mimic acoustic stimulation of a normal-hear
165  Subjects typically describe tinnitus in the deaf ear on the side of the surgery that can be modulate
166  1 of these, and was initially positive in 1 deaf ear, becoming negative at followup.
167 echanisms that regulate neuronal survival in deaf ears.
168                                  Va mice are deaf, exhibit circling behavior due to vestibular defect
169  ASL, and 16 hearing subjects who grew up in deaf families and were native ASL signers.
170 ing role of the mouth allows these seemingly deaf frogs to communicate effectively without a middle e
171              We tested children who had been deaf from birth and who subsequently received cochlear i
172            Here we demonstrate that in those deaf from birth the left and the right STC have altered
173  data revealed that in participants who were deaf from birth, STC showed increased activation during
174 ion of the auditory cortex develop in people deaf from birth?
175 ocessing when auditory processes are absent (deaf group) or impaired (dyslexic group).
176  sensory experience revealed less GMV in the deaf groups combined (compared with hearing groups combi
177 nstitutively expressing GFP (H9 Cre-LoxP) in deaf guinea pig cochleae that were pre-conditioned to re
178 knock-out mice (Atoh1(CKO)) are behaviorally deaf, have diminished auditory brainstem evoked response
179                                       In the deaf Heschl's gyrus, signal change was greater for somat
180     Therefore, cross-modal plasticity in the deaf higher-order auditory cortex had limited effects on
181 owever, both the auditory responsiveness of "deaf" higher-order fields and interactions between the r
182 man primary auditory cortex, in congenitally deaf humans by measuring the fMRI signal change in respo
183                          We conclude that in deaf humans the high-level auditory cortex switches its
184  and functional imaging) in a group of early deaf humans.
185 erties of dELL and a dELL-associated factor (dEaf) in a living organism.
186  in the deaf is complicated by the fact that deaf individuals also differ in their language experienc
187 aphy and psychophysics were assessed in tone-deaf individuals and matched controls.
188                        We found that sign in deaf individuals and speech in hearing individuals activ
189 aring developmental dyslexics and profoundly deaf individuals both have difficulties processing the i
190                                        These deaf individuals develop their own gestures, called home
191                Nevertheless, some profoundly deaf individuals do learn to read at age-appropriate lev
192                                        These deaf individuals have had no contact with any convention
193 d an analysis of nearly 5000 marriages among deaf individuals in America collected during the 19(th)
194                     Although the majority of deaf individuals in the United States are born to hearin
195 natomical and functional changes observed in deaf individuals is not only sensory, but also cognitive
196 common view, the "unused" auditory cortex of deaf individuals is reorganized to a compensatory sensor
197 suggest that during human motion processing, deaf individuals may engage specialized neural systems t
198 ditory feedback in vocal production, how can deaf individuals produce intelligible speech?
199                                 Congenitally deaf individuals receive little or no auditory input, an
200 ree data on 311 contemporary marriages among deaf individuals that were comparable to those collected
201  we focus on homesign, gestures developed by deaf individuals who cannot acquire spoken language and
202 s who lack conventional language for number (deaf individuals who do not have access to a usable mode
203 restored hearing in hundreds of thousands of deaf individuals worldwide.
204 or research areas: syntactic competence in d/Deaf individuals, and language documentation.
205                   In congenitally profoundly deaf individuals, auditory speech processing is essentia
206 d anxiety disorders occur at higher rates in deaf individuals.
207 t underlie reading achievement in profoundly deaf individuals.
208 uditory-based) representations of speech for deaf individuals.
209 calls for adjustments in the norms used with deaf individuals.
210 ture neural prostheses to restore hearing to deaf individuals.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT The question of
211 ly improves hearing thresholds in the mature deaf inner ear after delivery to nonsensory cells throug
212 ce of the brain's structural variance in the deaf is complicated by the fact that deaf individuals al
213  to underlie hair bundle degeneration in the deaf jerker mouse, subsequently leading to deafness.
214       However, PCDH15-CD2-deficient mice are deaf, lack kinociliary links and have abnormally polariz
215                                            A deaf lip-reading interpreter and a hearing American sign
216  first time, to elicit sound sensations in a deaf listener using an electrode implanted in his inner
217 of each syllable type emitted by hearing and deaf males in the presence of a female.
218 nal repair in the organ of Corti of a mature deaf mammal.
219                                     In these deaf mice, we found responses to noxious noise, which da
220 the pathogenesis processes in the cochlea of deaf mice.
221 lation of SGNs restored auditory activity in deaf mice.
222 ense phenotypic assortative mating among the deaf might have greatly accelerated the normally slow re
223 ction of wild-type VGLUT3 in the genetically deaf mouse cochlea results in significantly improved hea
224                                       In the deaf mutant quivering mouse, the localization of Nav1.6
225                                              Deaf native users of American Sign Language (ASL) were p
226 studies on deafness have been conducted with deaf native users of ASL (deaf signers).
227 wer areas of anatomical differences than did deaf native users of ASL (each compared with their heari
228 rience and language experience revealed that deaf native users of English had fewer areas of anatomic
229                                Pre-lingually deaf, native signers of ASL participated in the fMRI stu
230                        Children who are born deaf never have this bimodal experience.
231 ined the gesture systems that three isolated deaf Nicaraguans (ages 14-23 years) have developed for u
232      A new sign language has been created by deaf Nicaraguans over the past 25 years, providing an op
233                 A sample of young-adolescent deaf observers performed with higher accuracy than heari
234 for a general visual processing advantage in deaf observers rather than a face-specific effect.
235  Tshrhyt/hyt mutant mice remained profoundly deaf on P24 and although thresholds improved by approxim
236  with reference to studies of people who are deaf or bilingual.
237                              Persons who are deaf or hard of hearing face considerable barriers to co
238  between physicians and patients about being deaf or hard of hearing; different perceptions about wha
239 tle or no auditory input, and when raised by deaf parents, they acquire sign as their native and prim
240 ngless gestures (both relative to rest), the Deaf participants, but not the hearing, showed greater r
241 ith the same sign language experience as the deaf participants, did not activate the STCs.
242                                              Deaf participants, who used American Sign Language, acti
243 tivated to a greater extent by dyslexic than deaf participants.
244 d altered functions of left and right STC in deaf participants.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Those born deaf
245 g the appropriate regenerative therapy for a deaf patient.
246  [i.e., auditory midbrain implant (AMI)] for deaf patients who cannot benefit from cochlear implants
247                                     Thus, in deaf patients with GJB6 deletion as well as in the previ
248 ery of auditory function for some profoundly deaf patients, potential biological therapies must exten
249 guage has emerged among three generations of deaf people and their families in a Bedouin community in
250 contradictory views - that sign languages of deaf people are "just gestures," or that sign languages
251 ther the permanent auditory deprivation that deaf people experience leads to the enhanced visual proc
252                 Worldwide, more than 300,000 deaf people have been fitted with a cochlear implant; it
253 functioning auditory periphery of profoundly-deaf people to electrically stimulate their auditory ner
254                              Many profoundly deaf people wearing cochlear implants (CIs) still face c
255                                              Deaf people who use cochlear implants show surprisingly
256                      Here, we show that tone-deaf people, with impaired sound perception and producti
257 ially reorganize for face processing in born-deaf people.
258  neurons and recreate auditory sensations in deaf people.
259 s have provided hearing to more than 120,000 deaf people.
260 item duration or reduced memory abilities in deaf people.
261 sure waves observed during the developmental deaf period.
262  of many HCs with stereocilia these mice are deaf, possibly owing to HC and OC patterning defects.
263 marriage partners that included at least one deaf proband, who were ascertained by complete selection
264 maging (fMRI) to examine brain activation in deaf readers (N = 21), comparing proficient (N = 11) and
265                                   Proficient deaf readers activated left inferior frontal gyrus and l
266 e question of what differentiates proficient deaf readers from less-proficient readers is poorly unde
267                  We show that these mice are deaf secondary to rapid loss of initially well-formed ou
268 the question of whether observations made in deaf signers can be generalized.
269                                 As expected, deaf signers engaged left-hemisphere perisylvian languag
270                                    Data from deaf signers shows a non-uniform response to different c
271                     We tested two cohorts of deaf signers who acquired an emerging sign language in N
272 tations, this visuo-motor content can affect deaf signers' linguistic and cognitive processing.
273 een conducted with deaf native users of ASL (deaf signers).
274  the processing of non-linguistic actions in deaf signers.
275  1965 to 1997, and retinal examinations from deaf students born between 1985 and 1994, applying the W
276 ords, 148 from disability records, and 15 in deaf students.
277 tement: Successful restoration of hearing in deaf subjects by means of a cochlear implant requires a
278           Compared with both hearing groups, deaf subjects exhibited a significant increase in the am
279       Although performing the visual task in deaf subjects induced an increase in functional connecti
280              Intriguingly, we show that some deaf subjects perform faster than controls.
281  were based on MRI data from 25 congenitally deaf subjects who were native users of American Sign Lan
282  FC is retained to varying degrees among the deaf subjects, it may serve to predict the potential for
283 sk robustly activated the auditory cortex in deaf subjects, peaking in the posterior-lateral part of
284  gyrus, was activated to a greater extent by deaf than dyslexic participants, whereas the superior po
285 isual responses in Heschl's gyrus, larger in deaf than hearing, were smaller than those elicited by s
286  peripheral visual localization is better in deaf than in normal hearing animals, and that this enhan
287 how similar FC structure in the congenitally deaf throughout the auditory cortex, including in the la
288  duplication during Salmonella evolution, is deaf to SgrS because of a nonproductive G-U pair in the
289 een speakers with normal hearing and between deaf users of American Sign Language, but laughter rarel
290                                          For deaf users of ASL specifically, WMV differences resided
291                                          The deaf-waddler isoform of PMCA2, operating at 30% efficacy
292 mutant allele with mice heterozygous for the deaf-waddler mutant allele, we found severe hair bundle
293 ABR) showed that mtl and bsd homozygotes are deaf, whereas heterozygous and wildtype littermates have
294 g myosin IIIa and myosin IIIb are profoundly deaf, whereas Myo3a-cKO Myo3b(-/-) mice lacking myosin I
295               Some of these cats were raised deaf, whereas others were chronically implanted with coc
296                  Lcc/Lcc mice are completely deaf, whereas Ysb/Ysb mice are severely hearing impaired
297                                          The deaf white cat, a proven model of congenital deafness, w
298 ring until experimentation, and congenitally deaf white cats, which received no auditory inputs until
299             Slc26a4-null mice are profoundly deaf, with severe inner ear malformations and degenerati
300 osthetics, such as cochlear implants for the deaf, with very high spatial resolution.

WebLSDに未収録の専門用語(用法)は "新規対訳" から投稿できます。
 
Page Top