戻る
「早戻しボタン」を押すと検索画面に戻ります。

今後説明を表示しない

[OK]

コーパス検索結果 (1語後でソート)

通し番号をクリックするとPubMedの該当ページを表示します
1 tion deficits (WRD; sometimes referred to as dyslexia).
2 cale selection are impaired in developmental dyslexia.
3 sputedly the most efficient intervention for dyslexia.
4 fferences show causes rather than effects of dyslexia.
5 ains unknown, apart from a suggested role in dyslexia.
6 ysiology and reading skills in children with dyslexia.
7 nificantly predicted future reading gains in dyslexia.
8 , reliably predicted future reading gains in dyslexia.
9 an predict future long-term reading gains in dyslexia.
10 nisms underlying cerebral lateralization and dyslexia.
11 developmental disorders including autism and dyslexia.
12 clusion, a hallmark symptom in developmental dyslexia.
13 rity of children who would otherwise develop dyslexia.
14 -learning eye-movement control in hemianopic dyslexia.
15 nymous visual field disorders and hemianopic dyslexia.
16 ere impairment of reading, called hemianopic dyslexia.
17 el of current reading ability independent of dyslexia.
18 ative hand skill measure in individuals with dyslexia.
19  and related to atypical brain morphology in dyslexia.
20 obiological influences on the development of dyslexia.
21 ons of hypoactivation and hyperactivation to dyslexia.
22  most widely accepted areas of difficulty in dyslexia.
23 ocessing deficits are not a causal factor in dyslexia.
24 ptual and cognitive deficits associated with dyslexia.
25 ute early on to the phonological disorder in dyslexia.
26 otential relevance of these brain changes to dyslexia.
27 ility, or more specific neural correlates of dyslexia.
28 ain development are increasingly reported in dyslexia.
29  with developmental language disorder and/or dyslexia.
30 ar processing, contribute to the etiology of dyslexia.
31 nguage, spelling, and reading disability, or dyslexia.
32 nguage acquisition place a child at risk for dyslexia.
33 al intervention in adults with developmental dyslexia.
34  reading and picture naming in developmental dyslexia.
35 omal regions implicated in susceptibility to dyslexia.
36 g, spelling and language measures related to dyslexia.
37 gical processing in children and adults with dyslexia.
38 tic naming and the double-deficit subtype of dyslexia.
39 unctional neural mechanisms in children with dyslexia.
40  a neurobiological etiology of developmental dyslexia.
41 ccount for the perceptual errors observed in dyslexia.
42 ial programs can be effective in identifying dyslexia.
43 etween these neurobiological disruptions and dyslexia.
44  investigate such perceptual organization in dyslexia.
45 sual deficit in a global integration task in dyslexia.
46 kills, one of the key component processes in dyslexia.
47 f one of the genes involved in developmental dyslexia.
48 ts functional disconnection in developmental dyslexia.
49 mal subjects and subjects with developmental dyslexia.
50  been reported in the brain in developmental dyslexia.
51 ry has been linked to both schizophrenia and dyslexia.
52 rganization of the cortical visual system in dyslexia.
53  where dysfunction may lead to developmental dyslexia.
54 ecific computational deficit associated with dyslexia.
55 etion within the DCDC2 gene, a risk gene for dyslexia.
56 dulations, regardless of hereditary risk for dyslexia.
57 children may reveal early neural markers for dyslexia.
58 eptual learning as a factor in developmental dyslexia.
59 children at high and low hereditary risk for dyslexia.
60 le-brain functional connectivity analysis of dyslexia.
61 uals with language-related disorders such as dyslexia.
62 phrenia and that of structural correlates in dyslexia.
63 nto-parietal attention network characterizes dyslexia.
64  that weakness in V5/MT may not be causal to dyslexia.
65 EAD1" or "regulatory element associated with dyslexia 1."
66 r amyotrophic lateral sclerosis 5 (ALS5) and dyslexia-1 (DYX1) and TMOD4 a candidate gene for limb gi
67  children with a normal IQ and developmental dyslexia (16 male, three female; age range, 6-16 years;
68 tional MRI was performed on 20 children with dyslexia (8-12 years old) during phonological processing
69                       It is possible that in dyslexia a persistent sensory deficit in monitoring the
70  in modern societies, but many children have dyslexia, a difficulty in learning to read.
71                                              Dyslexia, a disorder of reading and spelling, is a heter
72       SD patients often present with surface dyslexia, a relatively selective impairment in reading l
73                                Developmental dyslexia, a severe and persistent reading and spelling i
74 nts in reading and writing characteristic of dyslexia, a view consistent with the recently appreciate
75    Language-based learning disorders such as dyslexia affect millions of people, but there is little
76                                Developmental dyslexia affects 5%-10% of the population, resulting in
77       Reviews on cortical visual impairment, dyslexia, Aicardi syndrome, and neuronal ceroid lipofusc
78                                Developmental dyslexia, an unexplained difficulty in learning to read,
79      We selected five pairs of children with dyslexia and (younger) typically developing readers matc
80 recent data on the genetics of developmental dyslexia and consider broader issues involved in the sea
81 15q11.2(BP1-BP2) deletion) have a history of dyslexia and dyscalculia, even after adjusting for IQ in
82 pen our understanding of the neural basis of dyslexia and highlight the importance of synchrony betwe
83 ms in three genes previously associated with dyslexia and implicated in neuronal migration (DYX1C1, D
84 e hypothesis for an M pathway abnormality in dyslexia and imply a strong relationship between the int
85 e hypothesis for an M pathway abnormality in dyslexia and imply strong relationships between the inte
86 r neural migration genes are associated with dyslexia and may contribute to auditory processing defic
87  reports of reduced behavioral adaptation in dyslexia and may reveal a difference in brain functions
88              DYX1C1 has been associated with dyslexia and neuronal migration in the developing neocor
89 useful to clarify the benefits of LCPUFAs in dyslexia and other closely related conditions.
90 en identified in patients with developmental dyslexia and psychomotor retardation.
91 X1C1) gene has recently been associated with dyslexia and reading scores in several population sample
92   Thus, in contrast to patients with surface dyslexia and semantic impairment from anterior temporal
93 vious studies have implicated this region in dyslexia and some speculations are made in this regard.
94 e of the KIAA0319 gene in the development of dyslexia and suggest that this gene influences reading a
95 t may be critical for reading improvement in dyslexia and that may differ from typical reading develo
96 n the temporo-parietal lobe in developmental dyslexia and that the altered cerebral structural symmet
97 f improvement was shallower for readers with dyslexia and the segment size where performance became a
98 phonological processing is characteristic of dyslexia and thought to be a basis for difficulty in lea
99 he relation between behavioral predictors of dyslexia and white matter organization in left arcuate f
100 sabilities, and learning difficulties (e.g., dyslexia) and were absent for sensory or motor/physical
101 in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, dyslexia, and fetal alcohol spectrum disorder.
102 m, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder, dyslexia, and other cognitive impairments, affect millio
103 ntify infants and young children at risk for dyslexia, and preventive intervention is often effective
104 h Omega-3 LC-PUFA found for ADHD, Dyspraxia, Dyslexia, and related conditions might extend to the gen
105       These results suggest that deficits in dyslexia are associated with a failure of the neural mec
106                                  Deficits in dyslexia are attributed to an intact declarative learnin
107 ncluding schizophrenia, autism, ADD/ADHD and dyslexia are believed to originate during gestation and
108 lecular evidence that cerebral asymmetry and dyslexia are linked.
109 y neuroanatomical abnormalities that precede dyslexia are not in the reading network itself, but rath
110 sults suggest that domains common to SSD and dyslexia are pleiotropically influenced by a putative qu
111                                 Readers with dyslexia are purported to have a selective visual impair
112                       Although the causes of dyslexia are still debated, all researchers agree that t
113 ns the motion processing deficit observed in dyslexia as the consequence of a lack, or poor quality,
114 ered neocortical activation, suggesting that dyslexia associated genes might play as yet unspecified
115       These results link the function of the dyslexia-associated gene Dcdc2 to spike timing through a
116                                  Variants in dyslexia-associated genes, including DCDC2, have been li
117     A matched control group of children with dyslexia attending the same schools who did not use the
118 egrity in kindergartners who are at risk for dyslexia because of poor phonological awareness.
119 rimary progressive aphasia and developmental dyslexia both manifest with phonological disturbances an
120 l magnocellular dysfunction is not causal to dyslexia but may instead be consequential to impoverishe
121 hat segmentation is impaired in readers with dyslexia but only on tasks containing motion information
122 ften have accompanying reading difficulties (dyslexia), but not all children with reading difficultie
123 sing deficits and language disorders such as dyslexia; but whether the former cause the latter, or si
124 h in children with and without developmental dyslexia by measuring auditory brainstem responses to a
125 tic basis of composite phenotypes related to dyslexia, by providing evidence for major-gene modes of
126 and picture naming deficits in developmental dyslexia can be reduced to a common neurological impairm
127 that the perceptual deficits associated with dyslexia can be understood computationally as a deficit
128                                Developmental dyslexia, characterized by unexplained difficulty in rea
129 hogenesis of such disorders as developmental dyslexia, congenital amusia and tinnitus.
130  An understanding of the role of genetics in dyslexia could help to diagnose and treat susceptible ch
131                                Developmental dyslexia (DD) and specific language impairment (SLI) are
132 ly studied candidate genes for developmental dyslexia (DD) owing to its important role in neuronal mi
133 h pedigree of eight cases with developmental dyslexia (DD) revealed several regions shared by the aff
134               Individuals with developmental dyslexia (DD) show a disruption in posterior left-hemisp
135    Furthermore, MGB activity correlated with dyslexia diagnostic scores, indicating that the task mod
136                                  People with dyslexia differ in their individual profiles across a ra
137 els for component phenotypes associated with dyslexia: digit span and a nonword-repetition task.
138 ction in disorders such as schizophrenia and dyslexia, diseases in which sex differences in incidence
139 ay be a mechanism for why some children with dyslexia do not respond to intervention.
140  speech (AOS), and left parietal symptoms of dyslexia, dysgraphia, and dyscalculia.
141 l dystrophy (DHRD), and one form of familial dyslexia (DYX-3).
142       Next we examine the cognitive model of dyslexia, especially the phonological theory, and review
143       Adults and children with developmental dyslexia exhibit reduced parietotemporal activation in f
144                             Individuals with dyslexia exhibited impaired voice-recognition abilities
145     In contrast, children with developmental dyslexia exhibited impairment in their ability to modify
146    For every stimulus type, individuals with dyslexia exhibited significantly diminished neural adapt
147 mulus repetition in adults and children with dyslexia for a wide variety of stimuli, spoken words, wr
148 paired reading development, individuals with dyslexia frequently exhibit behavioral deficits in perce
149 ional neuroimaging studies, individuals with dyslexia frequently exhibit both hypoactivation, often i
150 ognitive neuroscience measures could prevent dyslexia from occurring in the majority of children who
151  within the reading system; in developmental dyslexia, functional imaging is being used to identify t
152                    In patients with acquired dyslexia, functional imaging is demonstrating re-organis
153  demonstrate that suppression of a candidate-dyslexia gene causes deficits on tasks of rapid stimulus
154 ng Kiaa0319 RNAi and suggests that different dyslexia genes may cause different deficits in the speec
155 IC in the control group were not seen in the dyslexia group.
156  IFO-ILF in the control group but not in the dyslexia group.
157 of known reading regions (seeds) among three dyslexia groups characterized by (a) no remediation (cur
158 rontal gyrus was significantly weaker in all dyslexia groups, irrespective of remediation status/lite
159 ur results contribute to the hypothesis that dyslexia has a developmental neurobiological basis by li
160                                 Treatment of dyslexia has been advanced through neuroscience, yet fur
161 r to depend upon magnocellular pathways, and dyslexia has been associated with deficits in this pathw
162 he perception of rapidly presented sounds in dyslexia has been interpreted as evidence of a prolonged
163                                              Dyslexia has been reported in every culture studied, and
164 k involved in reading and its dysfunction in dyslexia has been well studied, it is unknown whether dy
165                Neuroimaging in children with dyslexia has revealed reduced engagement of the left tem
166 s resulting in selective reading difficulty (dyslexia) has remained elusive.
167                        Patients with surface dyslexia have disproportionate difficulty pronouncing ir
168 use of reading difficulties in developmental dyslexia; however, existing evidence also implicates deg
169 sess three leading theories of developmental dyslexia: (i) the phonological theory, (ii) the magnocel
170  typically developing readers, children with dyslexia improved their reading of novel words presented
171 ss gray matter volume (GMV) in developmental dyslexia in bilateral temporoparietal and left occipitot
172  functions and may be linked to epilepsy and dyslexia in humans.
173 vestigated the functional anatomy of surface dyslexia in SD using functional magnetic resonance imagi
174 ted by neuropsychological studies of surface dyslexia in semantic dementia and the connectionist tria
175 t of a focal cortical thickness reduction in dyslexia in the subregion of ventral occipitotemporal co
176 meliorate clinical conditions (e.g., autism, dyslexia) in which multisensory temporal function may be
177  of a QTL influencing multiple components of dyslexia, in particular the reading of irregular words (
178 ave identified several candidate regions for dyslexia, including one on chromosome 3 segregating in a
179                                              Dyslexia, incomplete homonymous hemianopia, preserved co
180 8p QTL is probably a general risk factor for dyslexia, influencing several reading-related processes.
181 uggesting future directions for the study of dyslexia intervention paradigms.
182 nce was examined to test the hypothesis that dyslexia involves a deficit in a specific visual pathway
183 behavioral plasticity in adult developmental dyslexia involves two distinct neural mechanisms, each o
184                                              Dyslexia is a common and complex disorder with evidence
185                                Developmental dyslexia is a common reading disorder that negatively im
186                                              Dyslexia is a developmental disorder in reading that exh
187                                Developmental dyslexia is a learning disability that specifically affe
188                                Developmental dyslexia is a neurodevelopmental condition which causes
189                                              Dyslexia is a neurodevelopmental disorder that is charac
190                                              Dyslexia is a prevalent reading disability whose underly
191                                Developmental dyslexia is a reading disorder, yet deficits also manife
192                  It is well established that dyslexia is a significantly heritable trait with a neuro
193                                Developmental dyslexia is a specific disorder of reading and spelling
194                                              Dyslexia is a specific impairment in reading that affect
195                                              Dyslexia is a specific learning disability that is neuro
196                                              Dyslexia is a widespread condition characterized by diff
197  the altered cerebral structural symmetry in dyslexia is associated with abnormal development of cell
198 d normal readers to test the hypothesis that dyslexia is associated with an abnormality in the magnoc
199 ortex, much in the same way as developmental dyslexia is associated with hypoactivation of this area.
200                   One hypothesis posits that dyslexia is caused by deficits in the motion processing
201 has been well studied, it is unknown whether dyslexia is caused by structural abnormalities in the re
202       Work confirms that, neurobiologically, dyslexia is characterised by dysfunction of the normal l
203                                Developmental dyslexia is characterized by a phonological processing d
204                                Developmental dyslexia is defined as a specific and significant impair
205                                     Although dyslexia is diagnosed through reading difficulty, there
206 blem in studying children with developmental dyslexia is how to separate inefficiency in learning on
207 uroimaging tests reviewed here indicate that dyslexia is indeed associated with cerebellar impairment
208                                              Dyslexia is one of the most prevalent childhood cognitiv
209  support a conclusion that the impairment in dyslexia is phonologic in nature and that these brain ac
210                           Our perspective on dyslexia is that some of the brain changes cause phonolo
211           A core difficulty in developmental dyslexia is the accurate specification and neural repres
212  has suggested that a fundamental deficit in dyslexia is the inability to process sensory input that
213                                              Dyslexia is the most common developmental language disor
214              Evidence for visual deficits in dyslexia is usually found only with dynamic and not stat
215 ilities (such as IQ, language impairment and dyslexia) is expected to provide new insights into the b
216                  Reading disability (RD), or dyslexia, is a common heterogeneous syndrome with a larg
217                  Reading disability (RD), or dyslexia, is a complex cognitive disorder manifested by
218         We first review the core concepts of dyslexia: its definition, prevalence, and developmental
219 eflected functional atypicalities related to dyslexia itself, independent of current reading ability,
220  The reported neuroanatomical differences in dyslexia may be causal to the reading problems, followin
221                                Children with dyslexia may read poorly for several reasons.
222               Some or all of the subtypes of dyslexia might have partly or wholly distinct genetic ca
223 n children with dyslexia (n = 25) or without dyslexia (n = 20) to discover whether initial behavioral
224 ngitudinal study over 2.5 y in children with dyslexia (n = 25) or without dyslexia (n = 20) to discov
225 AA0319, have been previously associated with dyslexia, neuronal migration, and ciliary function.
226                                              Dyslexia often arises from impaired phonological awarene
227                                Children with dyslexia often exhibit increased variability in sensory
228  identified a risk haplotype associated with dyslexia on chromosome 6p22.2 which spans the TTRAP gene
229 ral connectivity are related to the cause of dyslexia or if they are consequences of reading difficul
230 tnessed an explosion in our understanding of dyslexia (or specific reading disability), the most comm
231  four genes thus far linked to developmental dyslexia participate in brain development, and abnormali
232 tigates cortical signatures of developmental dyslexia, particularly from the perspective of behaviora
233  of variance); dark adaptation improved in 5 dyslexia patients after supplementation with a docosahex
234 g different reading experiences, rather than dyslexia per se, whereas the neuroanatomical precursors
235  fraction of the differences being driven by dyslexia per se.
236  a specific genotype, rather than the entire dyslexia population, contributing to the large variabili
237                                In studies of dyslexia, psychophysical and anatomical evidence indicat
238           Two other behavioral predictors of dyslexia, rapid naming and letter knowledge, did not cor
239 olymorphisms (SNPs) that was associated with dyslexia (reading disability) in two independent samples
240 e of relative hand skill in individuals with dyslexia [reading disability (RD)].
241 eptual distortions experienced by those with dyslexia reflect a disturbance in the basic mechanisms s
242      In contrast, areas of hypoactivation in dyslexia reflected functional atypicalities related to d
243            Thus, areas of hyperactivation in dyslexia reflected processes related to the level of cur
244  in the D6S464-D6S273 region for a number of dyslexia-related cognitive deficits.
245 enotypes were constructed to span a range of dyslexia-related cognitive processes.
246       The biological basis for developmental dyslexia remains unknown.
247 results indicate that the GMV differences in dyslexia reported here and in prior studies are in large
248 bility in impairment of motion thresholds in dyslexia reported in the literature.
249 other psychophysical tasks typically used in dyslexia research.
250         With the scientific underpinnings of dyslexia serving as a foundation, we turn our attention
251                                Children with dyslexia showed a correlation between the magnitude of i
252 eviously identified as enhancing the risk of dyslexia showed a reduced left-hemispheric asymmetry of
253               Physiologically, children with dyslexia showed increased activity in multiple brain are
254 nomalous expression of cerebral asymmetry in dyslexia similar to that of the planum temporale, which
255 ved across children with evidence of classic dyslexia, specific comprehension deficit, and language l
256 usceptibility locus and a linkage region for dyslexia, speech-sound disorder and reading.
257 regions in men with persistent developmental dyslexia, suggesting that the anatomical disconnection o
258                                One theory of dyslexia suggests that the phonological awareness defici
259                                          The dyslexia susceptibility 1 candidate 1 (DYX1C1) gene has
260 p quantitative-trait loci (QTLs) influencing dyslexia susceptibility have targeted specific chromosom
261 l cooperative associations may be present in dyslexia that are indicative of poor perceptual integrat
262 ith an increasingly sophisticated account of dyslexia that does not single out either phonological, o
263 n as a core neurophysiological difference in dyslexia that may underlie impaired reading development.
264 st a structural basis of behavioral risk for dyslexia that predates reading instruction.
265 ical mechanism leading to the development of dyslexia: the risk haplotype on chromosome 6p22.2 down-r
266 enge is to find ways that allow a child with dyslexia to read more words in less time, because readin
267                 We bring considerations from dyslexia to suggest that the claim can be extended furth
268                   From this, we propose that dyslexia training programs should take into account the
269  We studied the neural mechanisms underlying dyslexia using a simple frequency-discrimination task.
270  discrimination in children with and without dyslexia, using magnocellular and parvocellular visual s
271               Individuals with developmental dyslexia vary in their ability to improve reading skills
272 e formal literacy training began until after dyslexia was diagnosed.
273       In addition, the diagnosis of lifelong dyslexia was established by clinical means.
274        To investigate the pathophysiology of dyslexia, we used functional magnetic resonance imaging
275 r reading skills in adults and children with dyslexia were associated with greater repetition-induced
276 d may also have bearing on disorders such as dyslexia, which show sexual dimorphisms, and in which fu
277 sent data support the phonological theory of dyslexia, while acknowledging the presence of additional
278 g task and in young adults with a history of dyslexia who are well compensated for their disorder.
279 mining iFC can reveal cortical signatures of dyslexia with particular promise for monitoring neural c
280 zophrenia patients met criteria for acquired dyslexia, with 50% reading below eighth grade level desp
281 tential to reconcile influential theories of dyslexia within a predictive coding framework of brain f
282 cal processing predicted which children with dyslexia would improve reading skills 2.5 y later with >

WebLSDに未収録の専門用語(用法)は "新規対訳" から投稿できます。
 
Page Top