コーパス検索結果 (1語後でソート)
通し番号をクリックするとPubMedの該当ページを表示します
3 ngry (rostral anterior cingulate cortex) and fearful (amygdala and rostral anterior cingulate cortex)
4 ns, significantly reducing responses to both fearful and aggressive faces in face-responsive regions
5 ferior temporal cortex during the viewing of fearful and aggressive faces, but not during the viewing
6 la, thalamus, putamen and occipital areas to fearful and angry expressions at treatment follow-up com
9 asured threat-related amygdala reactivity to fearful and angry facial expressions using functional ma
10 o normally engage the amygdala in processing fearful and angry facial representations is more likely
13 y increased activation for emotional (mainly fearful and appeasing) faces compared with neutral faces
15 MRI was used to measure neural responses to fearful and calm faces presented preattentively (for 17
17 urotransmitter known to be linked to learned fearful and emotional behavior, has dual effects on exci
18 mygdala responses in G.Y. to presentation of fearful and fear-conditioned faces in his blind (right)
20 right parietal cortex distinguishes between fearful and neutral bodies as early as 80-ms after stimu
25 went functional MRI assessment while viewing fearful and neutral facial expressions at baseline and a
27 w that 7-mo-old infants discriminate between fearful and nonfearful eyes (experiment 1) and between d
29 task of negative emotional faces (angry and fearful) and geometric shapes that was designed for func
30 rain responses to threatening (ie, angry and fearful) and happy faces were examined as predictors of
31 ts to two types of social stimuli, negative (fearful) and positive (happy) emotional facial expressio
32 or no psychopathology (N=17) viewed neutral, fearful, and angry expressions while ostensibly making a
33 ouths) performed a labeling task with happy, fearful, and angry faces of varying emotional intensity.
34 dala activation in response to harsh (angry, fearful, and contemptous) vs accepting (happy) facial em
36 During fMRI scans, participants saw angry, fearful, and neutral expression stimuli while making a g
38 ain responses when observers viewed neutral, fearful, and scrambled faces, either visible or rendered
41 response to emotionally valenced faces (sad, fearful, angry, happy, neutral) following a negative moo
42 brain response to social signals of threat (fearful/angry faces) in 21 gSP patients before and after
43 otivated behavior becomes incrementally more fearful as the same microinjection is moved caudally.
45 rator in distress, an observer mouse becomes fearful, as indicated by a tendency to freeze and subseq
47 r are formed, and in the regions that convey fearful auditory information to the lateral nucleus.
48 , and equally prevented DNQX from generating fearful behavior (defensive treading) in caudal shell.
50 contributes to both appetitive behavior and fearful behavior that is generated in keyboard manner by
52 tions the disruptions generate progressively fearful behaviors (distress vocalizations, escape attemp
53 r example, either appetitive and/or actively fearful behaviors are generated in a keyboard pattern by
54 , but the disruption incrementally generates fearful behaviors as microinjection sites move more caud
55 erally suppressed both appetitive eating and fearful behaviors generated by NAc shell disruptions.
56 exually differentiated sexual, parental, and fearful behaviors in adults, this study examined the eff
58 ally placed disruptions produce increasingly fearful behaviors: distress vocalizations and escape att
59 , increasing the likelihood of aggressive or fearful behaviour in younger children, especially in boy
60 namics of automatic visual discrimination of fearful body expressions by monitoring cortical activity
63 ses, beginning 74-ms post-stimulus onset, to fearful, but not neutral or happy, facial expressions.
68 avioral cost of adaptation, specifically for fearful contents, demonstrating that aIPS contains a rep
74 mechanisms that may underlie the effects of fearful cue presentation, we measured release of [(3)H]-
76 which were trained to recognize a tone as a fearful cue, was suppressed at 2-3 h after exposure of a
78 ng, shock training (ST) and shock-associated fearful cues (FC) produce relatively selective decreases
83 nd extreme intensities of happy, sad, angry, fearful, disgusted, and neutral faces, balanced for gend
84 The authors investigated ASR modulation to fearful, disgusting, pleasant, and neutral stimuli in 12
89 e of a direct history of conditioning with a fearful event differs from directly learned avoidance.
90 e social ingroup and outgroup members with a fearful event, with the goal of advancing our understand
91 selecting the "fearful" category to describe fearful examples increased with experience and ranged fr
92 e that previously had been associated with a fearful experience (footshock) produces alterations in a
94 Neuromodulators released during and after a fearful experience promote the consolidation of long-ter
95 ere we show that during and directly after a fearful experience, new hippocampal representations are
98 neuron subtypes encoded distinct aspects of fearful experiences such as valence or value, whereas di
99 itecture, amygdala could enhance encoding of fearful expression movements from video and the form of
101 the greatest response in monkeys-even though fearful expressions are physically dissimilar in humans
105 ression analysis found amygdala responses to fearful expressions to be negatively associated with CU
106 inverse relationship between the response to fearful expressions under low attentional load and the c
107 traits showed reduced amygdala responses to fearful expressions under low attentional load but no in
108 increase in the typical amygdala response to fearful expressions under low relative to high attention
109 cortex responses to hybrid faces containing fearful expressions when such emotional cues are present
110 ts were less able than controls to recognize fearful expressions, and showed lower activation in pref
111 bjects and youths with ADHD while processing fearful expressions, but not neutral or angry expression
114 w a robust fear bias (increased attention to fearful eyes), their attention to angry and happy eyes v
116 e suggest that the conscious decision that a fearful face has been seen is represented across a netwo
118 creased amygdala-related connectivity during fearful face processing after the placebo treatment in h
120 f shock improved recall of threat-congruent (fearful) face location, especially in highly trait anxio
121 increased zygomaticus major activation) and fearful faces (leading to increased frontalis activation
122 es in sensitivity to the detection of masked fearful faces (whereby briefly presented, target fearful
123 gSP subjects reduced amygdala reactivity to fearful faces (which was exaggerated relative to HCs bef
124 citalopram on the left amygdala response to fearful faces (Z=2.51, p=0.027) and right amygdala respo
125 maging was used to detect brain responses to fearful faces and dynamic causal modelling was applied t
126 le paradigm that differentiates responses to fearful faces and fearful non-social images and (iii) me
127 amygdala and nucleus accumbens activation to fearful faces and lower nucleus accumbens activation to
128 a lack of startle potentiation while viewing fearful faces and showed reduced skin conductance respon
129 ful faces (whereby briefly presented, target fearful faces are immediately followed by a neutral face
131 both the visible and invisible conditions to fearful faces but much weaker in the invisible condition
133 ject-selective IT in response to unperceived fearful faces compared to unperceived nonface objects.
136 scanner, we manipulated visual awareness of fearful faces during an affect misattribution paradigm,
137 ants discriminated the gender of neutral and fearful faces filtered for low or high spatial frequenci
138 task designed to probe amygdala response to fearful faces following acute intranasal administration
139 tional connectivity during the processing of fearful faces in GSAD subjects and healthy controls (HCs
140 nconscious (backwardly masked) perception of fearful faces in healthy volunteers who varied in threat
141 at patient 1 showed potentiated responses to fearful faces in her left premotor cortex face area and
142 tween arousal and the cognitive appraisal of fearful faces in the condition of X-monosomy or Turner s
143 r frontal gyrus when inhibiting responses to fearful faces in the high-risk participants compared wit
144 selectively impairs explicit recognition of fearful faces in the presence of normal or enhanced auto
146 schizophrenia in response to presentation of fearful faces is paradoxically associated with failure t
148 ure, shorter mean fixation time when viewing fearful faces predicted higher PTSD symptom scores, and
149 ondingly, amygdala responses were greater to fearful faces presented at systole relative to diastole.
152 ivation of the "low road" subcortical route, fearful faces represent the only visually processed stim
154 gnificantly higher left amygdala response to fearful faces than healthy control subjects, whose activ
155 t not controls, showed greater activation to fearful faces than to happy faces in a distributed netwo
156 Overall, correct detection of angry and fearful faces was associated with greater activation com
158 r amygdala activation to the presentation of fearful faces was highly correlated with greater severit
159 visible, activity in FFA to both neutral and fearful faces was much reduced, although still measurabl
164 mygdala, where responses during appraisal of fearful faces were selectively reduced by carotid stimul
165 report high gamma (70-180 Hz) activation for fearful faces with earlier stimulus evoked onset in the
166 mited to low spatial frequency components of fearful faces, as predicted by magnocellular inputs to a
168 gdala response to unattended versus attended fearful faces, but "high-anxious" participants showed no
169 f unattended threat-related stimuli, such as fearful faces, has been previously examined using group
170 t patient 2, showed preserved recognition of fearful faces, intact modulation of acoustic startle res
171 esion typically show impaired recognition of fearful faces, this deficit is variable, and an intrigui
172 ivation evoked by repetitions of neutral and fearful faces, which were either task relevant (targets)
188 MRI paradigm that measures brain response to fearful faces; (ii) a fear-potentiated startle paradigm
193 suggest that while the amygdala can process fearful facial expressions in the absence of conscious p
194 mygdala volume and reduced responsiveness to fearful facial expressions observed in psychopathic indi
195 g object-emotion associations from happy and fearful facial expressions than it is to the presentatio
196 enhanced responsiveness of this structure to fearful facial expressions, an effect that predicts supe
197 tion, MDMA impaired recognition of angry and fearful facial expressions, and the larger dose (1.5 mg/
200 (ii) aggressive (open-mouthed threat), (iii) fearful (fear grin), and (iv) submissive (lip smack).
201 ns--neutral, aggressive (open mouth threat), fearful (fear grin), and appeasing (lip smack)--were pre
202 n their ability cognitively to differentiate fearful from other facial expressions but they acquire f
203 typal, paranoid), and the cluster C anxious, fearful group (obsessional, avoidant) became more pronou
205 block design incidental affective task with fearful, happy and neutral face stimuli and compared val
207 in a comfortable home environment to mostly fearful in a stressful environment, the roles of local D
208 in providing such a therapy, means that most fearful individuals are not able to receive the therapy
209 at old fear memories can be updated with non-fearful information provided during the reconsolidation
210 complex social behaviors such as shyness or fearful interaction with strangers can be observed, it m
212 2 tasks are quick to administer, involve no fearful learning associations, and require a simple appa
216 r, our data reveal that the consolidation of fearful memories related to simple auditory stimuli requ
217 her the auditory cortex is also required for fearful memories related to simple sensory stimuli.
219 tory cortex, is involved in the formation of fearful memories with a more complex sensory stimulus pa
223 s both appetitive motivation for rewards and fearful motivation toward threats, which are generated i
226 , and only in them did differential SCRs (to fearful-neutral faces) correlate positively with left fu
229 ifferentiates responses to fearful faces and fearful non-social images and (iii) measurement of skin
230 When a chimpanzee BT was interpreted as fearful, observers tended to underestimate the magnitude
233 ients prescribed topical glucocorticoids are fearful of side effects and fail to use them appropriate
234 bed an EAI were queried on whether they were fearful of using it and on factors that may contribute t
235 nals, such as face expressions, particularly fearful ones, and facilitates responses to them in face-
236 abeled neurons that were activated by either fearful or aggressive social encounters in a hypothalami
238 sual or auditory) with emotional expression (fearful or happy), we show that perceptual facilitation
242 y ranging in trait anxiety while they viewed fearful or neutral faces with averted or directed gaze,
244 stically different percent signal change for fearful or nonexpressive faces compared with the happy f
245 ferent populations of BLA neurons may encode fearful or rewarding associations, but the identifying f
246 scanned immediately following exposure to a fearful or safe context, and differences in [(18)F]fluor
249 ucing effect, one additional group of spider-fearful participants (n = 15) received a single dose of
253 s, reduced aggressive gestures, and enhanced fearful reactions to social cues compared with normal co
254 iety disorder showed increased activation to fearful relative to neutral expressions in several regio
255 rder showed significantly less activation to fearful relative to neutral faces compared to the health
256 ths had significantly greater activations to fearful relative to neutral facial expressions than did
259 ous individuals showed persistent, long-term fearful responses to both a HI and a model snake, alongs
260 la in mediating this endophenotype and other fearful responses, we prepared monkeys with selective fi
261 ers while they viewed a two-by-two matrix of fearful, sad, happy, and neutral facial expressions befo
262 ther positive incentive salience or negative fearful salience (valence depending on site and other co
263 table dimensions of responses (aggressive to fearful; shy to bold) across contexts and with a heritab
264 sensory consequences of vlPAG activation in fearful situations are well understood, but much less is
268 l magnetic resonance imaging, in response to fearful stimuli compared with individuals homozygous for
269 hose regions during perceptual processing of fearful stimuli demonstrated tight coupling as a feedbac
272 sorders tend to overgeneralize attributes of fearful stimuli to nonfearful stimuli, but there is litt
273 onance imaging study, amygdala reactivity to fearful stimuli was assessed in healthy male adults (n =
274 ty and on amygdala response while processing fearful stimuli were related to local availability of it
276 e in physiologic and behavioral responses to fearful stimuli, stressful stimuli, and drug-related sti
277 muli (t = 2.96, P = 0.006) (with a trend for fearful stimuli, t = 1.81, P = 0.08) compared with healt
278 ted with changes in the neural processing of fearful stimuli, we show activation of race-threat stere
283 In contrast, the same network responded to a fearful stimulus by enhancement of rhythmicity in the lo
284 he recovery of subjects' expectancies of the fearful stimulus is independent of when extinction occur
286 ed with subjects who denied fear of falling, fearful subjects had longer walk times, more comorbid co
287 ormed a go/no-go task responding to happy or fearful target faces presented in the left visual field,
288 Right fusiform activity was greater for fearful than neutral faces, independently of the attenti
291 ignificantly greater circuit coupling during fearful versus happy face processing in anxious, but not
292 teers had greater right amygdala activity to fearful versus neutral compared with happy versus neutra
293 ed fMRI to assess whether brain responses to fearful versus neutral faces are modulated by spatial at
294 on, having an increased amygdala response to fearful versus neutral faces regardless of attentional f
295 e right supplementary motor area during both fearful versus neutral, and happy versus neutral 'stimul
296 interested in how bimodal presentation of a fearful voice facilitates recognition of fearful facial
298 tended to exhibit diminished habituation of fearful vs happy responses in the right amygdala across
299 ly increased bilateral amygdala responses to fearful vs neutral faces (left p=0.025; right p=0.038 FW
300 Participants watched series of happy or fearful whole-body point-light displays (PLDs) as adapte
WebLSDに未収録の専門用語(用法)は "新規対訳" から投稿できます。