コーパス検索結果 (1語後でソート)
通し番号をクリックするとPubMedの該当ページを表示します
2 variant (n = 14, age = 61 +/- 9 years), non-fluent/agrammatic variant (n = 12, age = 71 +/- 8 years)
3 rontal aslant tract of patients with the non-fluent/agrammatic variant and in the uncinate fasciculus
4 longitudinal grey matter changes in the non-fluent/agrammatic variant of primary progressive aphasia
5 longitudinal atrophy progression in the non-fluent/agrammatic variant of primary progressive aphasia
6 The syndrome-specific epicentre of the non-fluent/agrammatic variant of primary progressive aphasia
7 s an essential supporting feature of the non-fluent/agrammatic variant of primary progressive aphasia
9 t longitudinal progression of atrophy in non-fluent/agrammatic variant primary progressive aphasia sp
10 a (eight with semantic variant, six with non-fluent/agrammatic variant, and four with logopenic varia
11 rior frontal cortex in patients with the non-fluent/agrammatic variant, and within the left temporo-p
14 we argue that highly efficient skills (i.e., fluent and highly accurate, "automatic," performance) ca
19 c dementia (SD) and six with progressive non-fluent aphasia (PA), as compared to 28 individuals with
20 totemporal dementia (bvFTD), progressive non-fluent aphasia (PNFA) (or a mixed aphasia) and semantic
22 The speech of patients with progressive non-fluent aphasia (PNFA) has often been described clinicall
24 D and the other in a case of progressive non-fluent aphasia (PNFA) without any apparent family histor
26 atively modality specific in progressive non-fluent aphasia and part of a more severe generic semanti
27 processing occurred in both progressive non-fluent aphasia and semantic dementia, and deficits of se
29 All patients developed a progressive non-fluent aphasia culminating in some cases in complete mut
31 impairment that was either a progressive non-fluent aphasia or decreased speech output consistent wit
32 rpersonal functioning, and patients with non-fluent aphasia overestimated emotional and interpersonal
33 f ARHGAP35 and SERPINA1 with progressive non-fluent aphasia point towards a potential role of the str
35 tia and, with one exception, progressive non-fluent aphasia were associated with transactive response
37 iant FTD, semantic dementia, progressive non-fluent aphasia, and FTD overlapping with motor neuron di
38 analysis were more common in progressive non-fluent aphasia, deficits of apperceptive processing occu
40 ith frontotemporal dementia, progressive non-fluent aphasia, semantic dementia or mixture of these sy
41 a clearer picture of cortical damage in non-fluent aphasia, the current study examined brain damage
50 put, damage to those regions results in non-fluent aphasic speech; when they are undamaged, fluent a
52 ional neuroimaging work with progressive non-fluent aphasics, compared directly to non-aphasic patien
56 tected between groups when all subjects were fluent-during both language formulation and non-linguist
57 his profile is different to that seen in the fluent form of primary progressive aphasia (fPPA), a neu
59 inguishing feature of Broca's aphasia is non-fluent halting speech typically involving one to three w
60 been dichotomized simply as 'fluent' or 'non-fluent', however fluency is a multidimensional construct
63 in catchment areas with a biological parent fluent in English or Spanish were enrolled from January
64 ' gestation, not using fertility treatments, fluent in English or Spanish, and available for telephon
65 atients age >/= 65 years with a solid tumor, fluent in English, and who were scheduled to receive a n
66 d a self-reported history of self-harm, were fluent in English, were medically fit to interview, and
67 e to women in remote villages; women who are fluent in Spanish are also more likely to present tricho
71 and a greater likelihood of having achieved fluent language (i.e., regular use of complex sentences)
73 The study included logopenic (n = 48), non-fluent (n = 54) and semantic (n = 96) variant primary pr
74 criteria divide PPA into three variants: non-fluent (nfvPPA), semantic (svPPA) and logopenic (lvPPA).
75 n tensor imaging in 48 individuals: nine non-fluent, nine semantic, nine logopenic and 21 age-matched
78 hasia has often been dichotomized simply as 'fluent' or 'non-fluent', however fluency is a multidimen
79 lus double IT arm did, however, exhibit less fluent output and were less effective at modulating thei
80 of diffusion tensor metrics alterations: non-fluent patients showed the greatest changes in fractiona
81 ges were found in the dorsal pathways in non-fluent patients, in the two ventral pathways and the tem
84 n = 15), fluent primary progressive aphasia (fluent PPA; n = 7), and amyotrophic lateral sclerosis wi
85 progressive aphasia (nonfluent PPA; n = 15), fluent primary progressive aphasia (fluent PPA; n = 7),
86 zheimer's disease, semantic dementia and non-fluent primary progressive aphasia (n = 9 each) were con
87 frontal operculum and caudate nucleus in non-fluent primary progressive aphasia (the corticobasal deg
88 1 year follow-up, all participants with non-fluent primary progressive aphasia had evolved either co
89 d behaviorally, early training produced more fluent processing of these stimuli than the same trainin
91 localizes to the regions of the brain where fluent reading occurs, and RNA interference studies show
92 ity is a brain-based difficulty in acquiring fluent reading skills that affects significant numbers o
93 /or language, and can be classified into non-fluent, semantic and logopenic variants based on motor s
96 guage, using electroencephalography (EEG) in fluent speakers of American Sign Language (ASL) as they
98 ariant sense to denote a subtype of PPA with fluent speech and impaired comprehension, even in the ab
99 nd insular atrophy; (2) SD, characterized by fluent speech and semantic memory deficits, was associat
100 peech entrainment allows patients to produce fluent speech by providing an external gating mechanism
101 osed that the acquisition and maintenance of fluent speech depend on the rapid temporal integration o
104 terior brain regions and may thus facilitate fluent speech production in individuals who stutter.
105 ic responses over neural regions integral to fluent speech production including inferior frontal gyru
106 The ability to express thoughts through fluent speech production is a most human faculty, one th
111 uage acquisition, segmentation of words from fluent speech, can be accomplished by 8-month-old infant
118 is may indicate the co-occurrence in the non-fluent variant of a deficit in working memory for audito
119 ), 20 behavioural variant FTD (bvFTD), 7 non-fluent variant PPA (nfvPPA), 6 semantic variant PPA (svP
120 primary progressive aphasia (svPPA), (4) non-fluent variant primary progressive aphasia (nfvPPA) or (
121 y progressive aphasia (svPPA), five with non-fluent variant primary progressive aphasia (nfvPPA)) and
122 otemporal dementia, semantic variant and non-fluent variant primary progressive aphasia, and 46 healt
124 Speech samples in patients with the non-fluent variant were characterized by slow rate, distorti
125 ctic errors were less common than in the non-fluent variant, while lexical access was less impaired t
127 terized by difficulties with accurate and/or fluent word recognition, spelling and decoding abilities
WebLSDに未収録の専門用語(用法)は "新規対訳" から投稿できます。