戻る
「早戻しボタン」を押すと検索画面に戻ります。

今後説明を表示しない

[OK]

コーパス検索結果 (1語後でソート)

通し番号をクリックするとPubMedの該当ページを表示します
1 onal shift to a 'high-drive' condition (18-h food deprivation).
2 crose pellets under 24-hr but not under 0-hr food deprivation).
3 nable weanling animals to survive periods of food deprivation.
4 drial beta-oxidation genes were repressed by food deprivation.
5 ct any alterations in ARC NPY mRNA following food deprivation.
6 ed by the refeeding hyperphagia that follows food deprivation.
7 5, 10, 20 and 40 microg) following overnight food deprivation.
8 ther of these responses when the stimulus is food deprivation.
9  sweeter nonnutritive sugar after periods of food deprivation.
10 in response to systemic/metabolic aspects of food deprivation.
11 ht to mediate the neuroendocrine response to food deprivation.
12  both physiological/psychological stress and food deprivation.
13  no inhibitory effects on feeding after 24 h food deprivation.
14 tion during the lengthening period of summer food deprivation.
15  or SHU9119 to rats before a 24 hr period of food deprivation.
16 elimination of odors as cues, and the use of food deprivation.
17 ody weight, and become hyperphagic following food deprivation.
18 o give rise to interoceptive cues like 24-hr food deprivation.
19 ity in other systems may be downregulated by food deprivation.
20 isual interneurons, an effect not altered by food deprivation.
21 failed to recover normal pumping rates after food deprivation.
22  did not show an altered feeding response to food deprivation.
23 ahepatic tissues during periods of prolonged food deprivation.
24 uced fibre atrophy caused by denervation and food deprivation.
25 to exhibit exaggerated locomotor response to food deprivation.
26 ll Period mutant mice was also robust during food deprivation.
27 ed by control floxed mice only after 24 h of food deprivation.
28 n than controls under conditions of complete food deprivation.
29 ing neurons increases in response to chronic food deprivation.
30 kness with ad libitum food and after 48 h of food deprivation.
31 y and suppressed 6-h refeeding after 24 h of food deprivation.
32  limited food is presented after a period of food deprivation.
33 bohydrate content of the diet consumed after food deprivation.
34 te for the reduced contribution from chronic food deprivation.
35 ally contiguous phases: 1-h chow access, 2-h food deprivation, 10-min chow access, and 10-min access
36               In Experiment 1, the impact of food deprivation (24 and 48 h) was examined.
37 ntal area upon feeding responses elicited by food deprivation (24 h), 2-deoxy-D-glucose-induced gluco
38 DAMGO (90% maximal decrease), and 24 h acute food deprivation (60% maximal decrease).
39 eus (Arc) and their synthesis increases with food deprivation, a naturally-occurring stimulus that ma
40                           Both restraint and food deprivation affected select limbic areas associated
41 etailed profile of cognitive consequences of food deprivation, affected by time of day, task demands,
42 e, and bacterial translocation compared with food deprivation (all p < 0.05).
43  on the GH-IGF-I axis differed from those of food deprivation alone; and b) whether administration of
44 culating FFA for glucose-stimulated IS after food deprivation also applies in the case of nonglucose
45                                              Food deprivation also challenges the gut microbiota, whi
46                                              Food deprivation alters the processing of sensory inform
47                 Included among these are (1) food deprivation and (2) acute microinjection of the neu
48                                              Food deprivation and concentration interacted to increas
49                               The effects of food deprivation and concentration on burst size (BS), b
50 he mammalian body provide resilience against food deprivation and dietary stress.
51 oth the depletion of energy induced by acute food deprivation and excessive storage of energy by high
52 bling induction of beta-oxidation genes upon food deprivation and facilitating activation of SCD in f
53 t small intestine OEA levels decrease during food deprivation and increase upon refeeding, suggesting
54 y a biological role for AGS-3 in response to food deprivation and indicate the mechanism for its acti
55 havioral shift occurs more rapidly following food deprivation and is modulated by cAMP and insulin si
56                                         Both food deprivation and MA (600 micromol/kg), but not 2-DG,
57 f the CRR, and thus the ability to withstand food deprivation and maintain euglycemia, is not known.
58                         However, the role of food deprivation and metabolic needs in the selection of
59 om environmental energetic stressors such as food deprivation and physical exertion.
60 Acb shell GABA(A) receptor stimulation or by food deprivation and produced a syndrome of forepaw trea
61 (dNPF), an ortholog of mammalian NPY, mimics food deprivation and promotes memory performance in sati
62 g despite normalizing feeding in response to food deprivation and PYY.
63 on the nutritional state-they decreased with food deprivation and recovered after subsequent feeding.
64 ationally related manipulations of the task (food deprivation and reward magnitude) produced some sub
65                 The ZI cells were excited by food deprivation and the gut hunger signal ghrelin.
66 multaneous variations in motivational state (food deprivation) and reinforcer magnitude (food present
67 iven to rats in a 'low-drive' condition (2-h food deprivation), and also after a motivational shift t
68   For example, ghrelin levels rise following food deprivation, and ghrelin administration stimulates
69 obility is regulated by caloric restriction, food deprivation, and heat shock.
70 muli, including exposure to dauer pheromone, food deprivation, and high temperature, can induce C. el
71 as markedly increased by lipin 1 deficiency, food deprivation, and obesity, often independent of chan
72 Twenty-eight peptides were increased >50% by food deprivation, and some of these were increased by 2-
73 tis elegans to alter several behaviors after food deprivation, apparently so that the animals can mor
74 food deprivation, suggesting that effects of food deprivation are mediated by modifying this protein.
75 severe childhood undernutrition (SCU) during food deprivation are not clear.
76 e increases in food intake following 24 h of food deprivation are reduced by systemic and central adm
77 s water maze without requiring foot shock or food deprivation as motivating factors.
78         Animals were studied after overnight food deprivation, awake, unrestrained, and unstressed; a
79 ate different protein breakdown responses to food deprivation between children with edematous and non
80 tabolic state, we examined whether overnight food deprivation blunts stress-induced recruitment of th
81        The results demonstrated that NPY and food deprivation both produced dose- or deprivation-depe
82             Peptides found to be elevated by food deprivation but not exercise included a number of f
83                                  Conversely, food deprivation but not NMDA injection produced marked
84                                     Finally, food deprivation, but not other stressors, stimulated cA
85 ly hatched Caenorhabditis elegans respond to food deprivation by halting development and promoting lo
86 of liquid diet or 10% sucrose solution after food deprivation by rats with APX also were considerably
87           Here we report that this effect of food deprivation can be blocked by leptin, a hormone inv
88                     These data indicate that food deprivation can provoke relapse to heroin seeking v
89  the delay was extended beyond 1 h, and that food deprivation could attenuate the degree of impairmen
90 gely on elevated responding to nonreinforced food deprivation cues.
91                                              Food deprivation decreased drinking latencies, did not c
92 by a state of negative-energy balance (24-hr food deprivation), demonstrating a specific influence of
93 duced rise in hypothalamic malonyl-CoA after food deprivation, demonstrating that leptin was not requ
94                                              Food deprivation did not affect BS but rather increased
95 esponse of chromatin-associated proteins, as food deprivation did not affect the mobility of heteroch
96             Following recovery from surgery, food deprivation discrimination performance was compared
97 lt mice, leptin was not acutely regulated by food deprivation during the early postnatal period.
98                                 We find that food deprivation elevates excitatory synaptic input, whi
99 lows survival during the frequent periods of food deprivation encountered during evolution.
100 s were tested for reinstatement after 1 d of food deprivation (experiment 1) or exposure to intermitt
101 ic gene expression resulting from short-term food deprivation (fasting) in Caenorhabditis elegans.
102                     We found that short-term food deprivation (fasting) potently induced p21 expressi
103 adapted to prolonged periods (1-2 months) of food deprivation (fasting) which result in metabolic cha
104 erian hamster feeding behaviors; (2) whether food deprivation (FD) co-increases agouti-related protei
105  completely blocked the effects of leptin on food deprivation (FD)-induced feeding.
106     EXPERIMENTS 2 AND 3: Following overnight food deprivation, five groups of rats were injected with
107                          We found short-term food deprivation for 24 h, already induces SIRT2 express
108                                              Food deprivation for 48 hr also increased CRE binding, w
109                                 In contrast, food deprivation had little effect.
110                                              Food deprivation has been shown to deleteriously affect
111 bing neurons controls behavioral response to food deprivation in C. elegans.
112 d tanycytes, were significantly decreased by food deprivation in male mice in a manner that was parti
113 nment in vivo, resulting in overeating after food deprivation in mice.
114 ke and body weight changes following 24 h of food deprivation in rats.
115 vation induced by 2-deoxy-D-glucose (2DG) or food deprivation in rodents.
116 lects calorie-rich foods following prolonged food deprivation in the absence of taste-receptor signal
117 mediating the short-term feeding response to food deprivation in the nucleus accumbens shell as well
118  of food and over an extended time course of food deprivation in wild-type and mutant worms to determ
119 immunoreactivity in this area is enhanced by food deprivation; in contrast, it is reduced by injectio
120 mice at this chronological age, because 24-h food deprivation increased arcuate NPY mRNA in wild-type
121                                              Food deprivation increased c-fos expression and spike fr
122 context of diet-induced and genetic obesity; food deprivation increased Deptor mRNA in both the ARC a
123                                              Food deprivation increases hypothalamic syndecan-3 level
124 easure of eating is modulated by satiety and food deprivation increases the reward value of food, the
125                       We also establish that food deprivation increases the worm's willingness to cro
126        MTII administration for 24 h prevents food deprivation-induced alterations in hypothalamic neu
127 nfused into the basolateral amygdala blocked food deprivation-induced changes in sucrose-related acti
128              In addition, IV-SUnSET detected food deprivation-induced decreases in PS in the heart, k
129   These similarities between NPY-induced and food deprivation-induced feeding are consistent with a s
130 oraging and food hoarding were not affected, food deprivation-induced increased food hoarding was sur
131  suggests an intact Arc is not necessary for food deprivation-induced increases in food foraging and
132                      Like obesity, prolonged food deprivation induces severe hepatic steatosis; howev
133                 Rats were trained to solve a food deprivation intensity discrimination problem in whi
134 lating concentration of FFA that accompanies food deprivation is a sine qua non for efficient GSIS wh
135 ce of energy and nutrient homeostasis during food deprivation is accomplished through an increase in
136  circulating FFA levels after 24 and 48 h of food deprivation is critically important for pancreatic
137                         Results suggest that food deprivation may act as a novel stress, thereby incr
138 er infusion of milk to assess the effects of food deprivation on sleep.
139    Lipid stores are mobilized in the case of food deprivation or high energy demands--for example, du
140 ntaining neurons or fibers are unaffected by food deprivation or MA, and the antimetabolite 2-DG has
141 ine [TMT]), a paradigm that does not involve food deprivation or operant performance.
142                   Here, we show in mice that food deprivation or optogenetic activation of AgRP neuro
143 hich can be altered by manipulations such as food deprivation or prior exposure to MOP receptor agoni
144  stimuli (footshocks, shock-predictive cues, food deprivation, or reward omission) and inhibitions af
145 learned schedules of food intake rather than food deprivation per se.
146 at another environmental stressor, acute 1 d food deprivation, potently reinstates heroin seeking in
147 ess NMDA-elicited feeding by PTK inhibitors, food deprivation readily drives PTK activity in vivo.
148                                        Acute food deprivation reinstated heroin seeking, an effect th
149  corticosterone levels were reduced, whereas food deprivation resulted in significantly enhanced plas
150 In Caenorhabditis elegans, pairing odor with food deprivation results in aversive olfactory learning,
151  in which stimuli produced by 0-hr and 24-hr food deprivation served as discriminative cues for the d
152                     Here, we show that after food deprivation, SIRT1 levels fall dramatically in type
153 ternation in a T-maze and on a task in which food-deprivation state was used as a contextual cue.
154 effects on food intake both under normal and food deprivation states.
155 ine or sucrose, it effectively blocked acute food deprivation stress-induced reinstatement of cocaine
156  results independent from changes related to food deprivation stress.
157 change in its biochemical fractionation upon food deprivation, suggesting that effects of food depriv
158 , whereas nutrient supplementation following food deprivation suppressed it; the former and latter co
159                                              Food deprivation suppresses animal growth and developmen
160                                              Food deprivation suppresses sleep, presumably to increas
161                                    Following food deprivation, the expression and acetylation of the
162                               In response to food deprivation, the pumping rate rapidly declines by a
163 responses that are otherwise associated with food deprivation; thus, unlike forced dieting, cessation
164 at an increase in locomotor speed induced by food deprivation was accompanied by an activity- and oct
165                                 In addition, food deprivation was tested as a possible modulating fac
166 portance of considering prolonged periods of food deprivation when assessing chemical risks posed to
167 ach 4-min session that took place under 0-hr food deprivation, whereas no pellets were delivered duri
168                                              Food deprivation which enhances aPVN GAL produces a mark
169 other, naturally occurring stressors such as food deprivation, which is being exacerbated by global w
170                                              Food deprivation, which transiently favors feeding over
171 /DA and HVA/DA ratios were normalized by age/food-deprivation while that of 3MT/DA was not.
172 ice, otherwise apparently normal, respond to food deprivation with markedly reduced reflex hyperphagi
173 crose palatability induced by an increase in food deprivation without affecting the performance of su

WebLSDに未収録の専門用語(用法)は "新規対訳" から投稿できます。
 
Page Top