コーパス検索結果 (1語後でソート)
通し番号をクリックするとPubMedの該当ページを表示します
1 e during two distinct movements (running and grooming).
2 e sufficient to specifically elicit antennal grooming.
3 ly occurring cooperative interaction: social grooming.
4 were preferentially active during eating and grooming.
5 and pup-directed aggression and induces pup grooming.
6 cial interactions with conspecifics, such as grooming.
7 affolding gene, Sapap3, results in excessive grooming.
8 ence mimicking effects of rodent licking and grooming.
9 ed relatively high levels of repetitive self-grooming.
10 the ventral striatum resulted in repetitive grooming.
11 behaviors, and an acute 12-fold increase in grooming.
12 ow decreased pausing, hanging, drinking, and grooming.
13 o natural rewards such as sucrose and social grooming.
14 nt for eliciting stress-induced anorexia and grooming.
15 but not CeA, decreases feeding and increases grooming.
16 d evoke escape responses, and both stimulate grooming.
17 CNS previously implicated in the control of grooming.
18 from a chair, using the toilet, eating, and grooming.
19 d that it was sufficient to induce excessive grooming.
20 vities such as scratching, yawning, and self-grooming.
21 sis of rear limbs, hunched posture, and poor grooming.
22 n in the area of skin targeted for excessive grooming.
23 bouts of AVP-induced flank marking and flank grooming.
24 , and D1-like agonists selectively stimulate grooming.
25 uency of either flank marking or flank gland grooming.
26 cal movements, such as chewing, licking, and grooming.
27 hair grooming and 538 (16.2%) reported never grooming.
28 yopia) in old wild bonobos, exhibited during grooming.
29 s and motivations associated with pubic hair grooming.
30 approximately 22 cases per 100 persons) and grooming (17-19% vs. 7-15%; attributable disability, app
31 , 5674 of 7456 (76.1%) reported a history of grooming (66.5% of men and 85.3% of women [weighted perc
36 entral pallidum and globus pallidus impaired grooming actions but left the sequential organization of
38 increase of exploratory motor behaviors and grooming activity also is observed, consistent with a gl
42 ce with GRN mutations displayed OCD and self-grooming (an OCD-like behavior in mice), respectively.
44 otine exposure, female rats showed decreased grooming, an effect not seen in males; this effect is op
48 elationship between variation in pup licking/grooming and arched-back nursing (LG-ABN) and offspring
50 nt behaviour patterns, including tool usage, grooming and courtship behaviours, are customary or habi
52 ge behaviours including locomotion, rearing, grooming and drinking for up to 7 weeks post occlusion,
53 pproximately equal levels of activity during grooming and eating and maximal activity during explorat
56 nies and inhabiting temporary nest bivouacs, grooming and feeding with workers, but also consuming th
58 unexpected behavior manifested by compulsive grooming and hair removal, similar to behavior in humans
60 T+ Itpr3tf/J (BTBR), reduced repetitive self-grooming and high marble burying scores in BTBR, and red
61 imb stereotypy, hindlimb clasping, excessive grooming and hypo-activity prior to death between 7 and
64 s in mice, which manifests as excessive self-grooming and increased anxiety-like behaviors, and is al
65 tion of mGluR5 activity attenuates excessive grooming and instrumental learning differentially, and r
66 However, maternal simulation of pups (e.g., grooming and milk ejection) consistently produced rapid,
67 f central nervous system processes including grooming and nursing, depression and stress, and drug ab
68 ny type of recent interaction, because prior grooming and prior aggression influenced their behavior
71 tion of MC4R signaling, normalize compulsive grooming and striatal electrophysiologic impairments in
72 ice and then transferred to the pelage after grooming and subsequently to the environment allowing an
73 PGRN knockout mice abolished excessive self-grooming and the associated hyperexcitability of medium
74 urvey (two summers, one with and one without grooming) and a 48 h survey during the first ever intens
75 investigation, contact time), comfort (i.e., grooming), and locomotion (i.e., contact time, cage clim
76 sulted in a significant decrease in freezing grooming, and climbing and caused a significant increase
77 ed deficits in social interaction, excessive grooming, and decreased exploration of a novel environme
81 ecifics such as sexual behavior, yawning, or grooming, and not as much-as is often observed in humans
82 on infants' social interactions (aggression, grooming, and play) and self-scratching (a proxy indicat
84 ring development, such as postpartum licking/grooming, and that effects of Peg3 are dependent on the
86 tion of the KO mice exhibited high levels of grooming; and (2) the average duration of nose contact w
87 valuated for OCD-relevant phenotypes of self-grooming, anxiety-like behaviors, and increased striatal
88 reases in sniffing, rearing, locomotion, and grooming as well as by a tendency to turn to the ipsilat
90 t with more bystanders, initiators gave less grooming at the beginning of the bout and were more like
91 iors, including the progression of excessive grooming behavior (n=8) and hypersensitivity to mechanic
92 ial challenges (male immigration, changes in grooming behavior after the death of a close relative, a
93 and cold allodynia, combined with excessive grooming behavior have been shown to be the behavioral c
94 entrations were associated with increases in grooming behavior in rats treated with the highest conce
96 , tremors, jumps, rears, wet-dog shakes, and grooming behavior precipitated by subcutaneous administr
99 dramatic increase in repetitive, stereotyped grooming behavior, a potential autism-relevant abnormali
100 in the dorsal striatum (DSt), alterations in grooming behavior, and enhanced sensitivity to the stere
105 and the Slitrk5-deficient mice, the abnormal grooming behaviors are associated with enhanced anxiety
107 of novel mouse genetic models with excessive grooming behaviors have begun to shed light on the molec
110 rease in prepulse inhibition, an increase in grooming behaviors, an impairment in nest-building activ
112 s limited information about finer aspects of grooming behaviors, which are important to understand mo
116 ng social interaction deficit and repetitive grooming behaviour could be rescued, while anxiety and m
117 ap3 exhibit increased anxiety and compulsive grooming behaviour leading to facial hair loss and skin
118 f the bout and were more likely to abandon a grooming bout, while bouts were less likely to be recipr
121 ocial communication through touch and mutual grooming can convey highly salient socio-emotional signa
126 rupt the syntax or serial order of syntactic grooming chains without disrupting constituent movements
128 symptoms of withdrawal measured as increased grooming, chewing, scratching, and shaking, plus the app
131 esting that contrary to current assumptions, grooming decisions are not based on trust, or bonds, wit
138 ind differences in the durations of antennal grooming elicited by neurons in the different layers, su
140 oring the time of onset and duration of each grooming episode, and are often performed on grooming ep
141 grooming episode, and are often performed on grooming episodes triggered by stress exposure, which ma
146 ons suggest that the excessive, pathological grooming exhibited by these mice results from CNS abnorm
147 ngham et al.[1] - the proportion of high-arm grooming featuring palm-to-palm clasping - we found that
148 atecholamine plasma levels while stimulating grooming, feeding behaviors, gastric transit and acid se
151 of grooming, hairiness, instrument used, and grooming frequency, men who removed all their pubic hair
157 gham et al.[1] reduced the cultural scope of grooming-handclasp behavior by showing that grooming-han
158 y corroborating our original conclusion that grooming-handclasp behavior can represent a group-level
159 grooming-handclasp behavior by showing that grooming-handclasp style convergence is "explained by ma
160 previously reported cultural differences in grooming-handclasp style preferences in captive chimpanz
161 of sensorimotor events including locomotion, grooming, head movement, chewing, auditory stimuli, and
163 otonin reuptake inhibitor, reduces excessive grooming, hyperanxiety and social behavioral impairments
164 in mice, which display similar pathological grooming, hyperanxiety and social impairment deficits.
165 howing an increased frequency of pup licking/grooming (i.e., high-LG mothers) that was associated wit
177 Post-natal behaviours, notably licking and grooming in rats, cause decreased behavioural indices of
181 data show STN-HFS suppresses excessive self-grooming in two autism-like mouse models, raising the po
182 asping (PPC) is a distinct style of high-arm grooming in which the grooming partners clasp each other
183 High-arm grooming is a form of chimpanzee grooming in which two individuals mutually groom while e
184 ips strongly influence the style of high-arm grooming in wild chimpanzees of the Kanyawara community.
185 DOPA treatment produced gradually sensitized grooming in WT mice but failed to induce any sensitized
187 A of maternally deprived rats, and surrogate grooming increased CRF2mRNA expression significantly abo
188 compulsive-like behaviors such as compulsive grooming increased in frequency by the end of the mangan
190 ntake, enhanced pain response, and excessive grooming induced by intracerebroventricular NMU administ
193 ing their lifespan had an increased risk for grooming injury (adjusted odds ratio [AOR], 1.97; 95% CI
201 ing from <5% (M group, Mahale) to >30% dyads grooming (Kanyawara, Kibale), and in a large free-rangin
204 ous treatment caused poor tolerability (poor grooming, lethargy) and significantly higher day-14 RAD
208 d, prepro-TRH178-199 significantly increased grooming, locomotor activity, rearing, and sniffing beha
209 red with high levels of maternal licking and grooming (low LG offspring) in early postnatal life show
211 owever, Gnal haploinsufficiency altered self-grooming, motor coordination, and apparent motivation in
212 nt trace of this excitation induces the next grooming movement once the previous one is performed.
213 quenced (syntactic) pattern of cephalocaudal grooming movements (CCGs) shared by all rodent suborders
215 uring syntactic chains compared with similar grooming movements made in different sequential order or
218 ct mouse models demonstrating excessive self-grooming, namely the Viaat-Mecp2(-/y) and Shank3B(-/-) l
219 ents in fine- and sensorimotor tasks such as grooming, nest building and acoustic startle as early as
226 sure prior to the birthing process, maternal grooming, or encounters with contaminated environments.I
227 the recruitment call of an unrelated recent grooming partner caused subjects to move in the directio
228 ts were most likely to approach their former grooming partner when they had also heard her recruitmen
233 er demonstrate that the increased repetitive grooming phenotype can be rescued in adult mice by admin
234 euronal survival and results in an excessive grooming phenotype caused by dysregulation of Sap90/Psd9
235 these two phenotypes are separable, with the grooming phenotype derived from the hematopoietic lineag
236 ing number of rodent models that have strong grooming phenotypes, this high-throughput in-depth analy
237 g smaller, less diverse, and less integrated grooming, play, proximity and contact-sitting networks.
244 r, the frequency of active behaviors such as grooming, rearing, burrowing and locomotion increased.
247 oral risk factors associated with pubic hair grooming-related injuries to characterize individuals wi
251 lude antibiotic glandular secretions, mutual grooming, removal of diseased individuals from the nest,
253 as non-aggressive biting of cagemates during grooming, repeated leaping and episodes of perseverance
254 riments, young crayfish exhibited a directed grooming response to all worm species, but were unable t
257 odel whereby suppression drives a Drosophila grooming sequence that is induced by simultaneous activa
258 tion of different sensory pathways elicits a grooming sequence that resembles the naturally induced s
261 ooming, support the hypothesis that antennal grooming serves a similar function in a wide range of in
262 lth care professionals and those who provide grooming services can use this information to better cou
263 ine, and octopamine stimulate locomotion and grooming, showing distinguishable effects that often are
264 ide array of behaviors (locomotion, rearing, grooming, stereotypies) including a microstructural anal
265 and drinking while concomitantly increasing grooming, stereotypies, and ethological plus traditional
268 effort to assess whether the transmission of grooming styles within two captive groups in Chimfunshi
269 ks function to coordinate and prolong social grooming, suggesting that this oral signal is an example
270 tica), which use different modes of antennal grooming, support the hypothesis that antennal grooming
277 features (for example, walking, turning and grooming), thus producing the highest-resolution etholog
279 ogaster), greatly increases partner-directed grooming toward familiar conspecifics (but not strangers
283 Neuronal activation stimulated by induced grooming was accompanied by an increase in total glutama
288 tivity, as measured by center crossings, and grooming were also reduced by subchronic treatment with
293 ly to produce lip-smacks during face-to-face grooming where the visual aspect of the signal could be
294 responding for high-fat diet and compulsive grooming, whereas group-housed female mice display incre
295 -specific deletion of Mecp2 causes excessive grooming, which is rescued by restoring striatal Sapap3
296 independently of its effect to promote self-grooming, while the GABAergic subpopulation inhibits sel
297 al behaviors including adult-onset excessive grooming with mild-to-severe hair loss and self-injury.
298 etime show coordinated femur movement during grooming with one hindleg, suggesting that grooming may
299 ties, samples of at least 100 observed dyads grooming with raised hands showed PPC frequencies varyin
300 Race was also significantly associated with grooming, with all groups reporting less grooming when c
WebLSDに未収録の専門用語(用法)は "新規対訳" から投稿できます。