コーパス検索結果 (1語後でソート)
通し番号をクリックするとPubMedの該当ページを表示します
1 dent circuits to learn distinct aspects of a motor skill.
2 properties that enable robust learning of a motor skill.
3 rtant input for successful learning of a new motor skill.
4 tudying the plasticity associated with a new motor skill.
5 erm change, that together constitute the new motor skill.
6 sulcus following training of a complex visuo-motor skill.
7 creased white matter volume predicted poorer motor skill.
8 eased white matter volume predicted improved motor skill.
9 t to the control of song, a complex, learned motor skill.
10 elated to the development and maintenance of motor skill.
11 nt practice, or recall of the newly acquired motor skill.
12 Birdsong is a learned, sequenced motor skill.
13 nt specialization of the left hemisphere for motor skill.
14 rtex of two monkeys while they learned a new motor skill.
15 initial training through mastery of a simple motor skill.
16 umented, reflect failure of adaptive complex motor skills.
17 new OLs and myelin is important for learning motor skills.
18 dback responses to the requirements of novel motor skills.
19 ove daily activities, walking, exercise, and motor skills.
20 ack responses should adapt when we learn new motor skills.
21 acquisition, consolidation, and retention of motor skills.
22 are impaired in motor tasks that require new motor skills.
23 for the development of both intellectual and motor skills.
24 on-line, or stores and produces well-learned motor skills.
25 's disease (PD) have difficulty learning new motor skills.
26 underlies the acquisition and maintenance of motor skills.
27 an important role in memory consolidation of motor skills.
28 is proposed to fine tune movment and improve motor skills.
29 visual memory, visuospatial perception, and motor skills.
30 the transition from novel actions to refined motor skills.
31 opportunities to reshape previously learned motor skills.
32 of the scores based on domains that measure motor skills.
33 ant reduction in the capability of acquiring motor skills.
34 tis virus-induced demyelination and impaired motor skills.
35 its potential consequences on perceptual and motor skills.
36 han nonabusers on functions of attention and motor skills.
37 ticospinal circuits to orchestrate multistep motor skills.
38 ral mechanisms underlying the acquisition of motor skills.
39 ption may influence performance in untrained motor skills.
40 n excitatory synapses and the acquisition of motor skills.
41 or cortical interactions subserving adaptive motor skills.
42 euronal replay in the consolidation of novel motor skills.
43 interfere with learning of new, but related, motor skills.
44 transfers to subsequent learning of related motor skills.
45 ssociated with adverse outcome for both fine motor skills (25.0% vs 4.8%, respectively; odds ratio =
46 3]; language, 49.2 vs 54.4 [P < .001]; gross motor skills, 48.7 vs 53.0 [P = .002]; pain/itching, 15.
47 ed up motor skill acquisition and to improve motor skill accuracy, as well as to further our understa
48 rebellar tDCS as an intervention to speed up motor skill acquisition and to improve motor skill accur
55 During sequence learning, individuals show motor-skill acquisition and an ability to verbally descr
57 ted while, for example, performing a complex motor skill adds complexity to a task and thus leads to
59 es demonstrating offline behavioral gains in motor skills after sleep, the underlying neural mechanis
60 ptic drugs was associated with impaired fine motor skills already at age 6 months, especially when th
63 predominately after the acquisition of a new motor skill and that it is related to changes in memory
64 IB]) that tests posture, tone, reflexes, and motor skills and a visual habituation paradigm using a n
66 irst two tasks suggested comparable sensory, motor skills and contextual memory in all three groups.
67 ebral artery occlusion, both the recovery of motor skills and corticofugal axonal plasticity are prom
69 uired to understand how the brain learns new motor skills and ensures existing behaviors remain appro
70 test scores increased from 0.30 to 1.98 for motor skills and from 0.90 to 1.98 for process skills.
71 t role in the acquisition and maintenance of motor skills and in the effects of spinal cord injury an
72 ental retardation, loss of communication and motor skills and infantile spasms and seizures in predom
73 tic plasticity are critical for learning new motor skills and maintaining memory throughout life, whi
74 h hemisphere drives postnatal development of motor skills and stable CS tract connections with contra
75 ly identification of deficient oropharyngeal motor skills and vocal cord dysfunction is crucial to es
79 , childhood deficits in verbal memory, gross motor skills, and attention identified 83%, 75%, and 58%
81 brain leads to impairments in cognitive and motor skills, and is the major risk factor for several c
82 , verbal memory, psychomotor speed, and fine motor skills, and sCD164 remained associated with execut
83 Alcohol intoxication alters coordination and motor skills, and this is responsible for a significant
86 e indicates that intellectual and perceptual-motor skills are acquired in fundamentally similar ways.
89 sion that intellectual skills and perceptual-motor skills are psychologically more alike than differe
90 learning of motor sequences, suggesting that motor skills are stored in the sensorimotor territory of
91 velops during the later phases of refining a motor skill as the actor becomes sensitive to the outcom
92 AC5KO mice were impaired in acquisition of a motor skill, as assessed by the accelerated rotarod.
93 d subjects diagnosed with ASD underwent fine motor skill assessment and scanning with diffusion tenso
95 ignificantly higher cognition, language, and motor skills at 4 years of age than children who did not
98 rly on neurocognitive tests of attention and motor skills, both factors that can adversely affect tre
99 important for acquisition and maintenance of motor skills, but how the loss of dopamine in PD leads t
100 to underlie the acquisition and execution of motor skills, but its contributions to these processes a
101 lthough walking is a well-practiced, refined motor skill by late childhood (i.e., 11 years of age), t
102 tested whether the acquisition of a complex motor skill can be enhanced in old subjects by the appli
103 sults indicate that variable sequencing in a motor skill can reflect an end point of learning that is
106 vidence suggests that the acquisition of new motor skills can directly influence later visual percept
109 d movements in maturity, it is accepted that motor skills cannot occur until the CST develops a matur
110 (n = 223) had a higher risk of impaired fine motor skills compared with the reference group (11.5% vs
112 rovide a key demonstration that consolidated motor skills continue to change as needed through the re
115 roups experienced more perinatal insults and motor skill deficits, caretaker instability, criminality
119 ur results demonstrate that a newly acquired motor skill depends on the formation of a task-specific
120 he acquisition and life-long preservation of motor skills depends on continual adaptive plasticity th
124 levodopa treatment after acquisition of the motor skill does not result in an immediate drop in perf
126 ing play, language, fine motor skills, gross motor skills, emotional behavior, family functioning, pa
130 ays an important role in learning of complex motor skills, from learning to serve in tennis to perfec
131 OQ0-5 domains including play, language, fine motor skills, gross motor skills, emotional behavior, fa
135 W5) to PW7] produces permanent contralateral motor skill impairments, loss of M1 motor map, aberrant
137 ural consolidation, expressed as an off-line motor skill improvement, can be blocked by declarative l
142 le rats while they were trained with a gross motor skill in which they learn to maintain their balanc
146 roduction of reproductive signals and sexual motor skills in many vertebrates; therefore, one possibi
147 hat preserved learning of complex perceptual-motor skills in patients with amnesia is a robust phenom
148 Our results suggest that learning a novel motor skill induces structural change in task-relevant W
149 sorder include loss of acquired language and motor skills, intellectual impairment and hand stereotyp
151 l ganglia neuronal ensembles bring automatic motor skills into voluntary control and integrate them i
152 neuroimaging studies that the acquisition of motor skills involves both perceptual and motor learning
155 oural studies indicate that a newly acquired motor skill is rapidly consolidated from an initially un
158 ariability in the execution of movements and motor skills is ubiquitous and widely considered to be t
159 ellum does not contribute to learning of the motor skill itself but is engaged primarily in the modif
161 dissociable plasticity during fast and slow motor skill learning and suggest that distinct neural pr
163 mplifies general processes of perceptual and motor skill learning and, more specifically, resembles h
165 ssociative learning and the initial phase of motor skill learning are ensured by feedback-based mecha
166 one of three conditions: (1) an "acrobatic" motor skill learning condition (AC), (2) a motor activit
170 training, our results suggest that acrobatic motor skill learning involves a reduction of some PF inp
172 sue of whether the cerebellum contributes to motor skill learning is controversial, principally becau
176 We find that fmr1 KO mice have impaired motor skill learning of a forelimb-reaching task, compar
178 y diminished during training on an acrobatic motor skill learning task, largely because of reduced ad
180 f processes, ranging from the fine-tuning of motor skill learning to important social functions, such
182 ent spatial and temporal scales that mediate motor skill learning while identifying converging areas
183 t motor learning mechanisms, its role during motor skill learning, a behavior that likely involves er
186 ic spine formation in the mouse cortex after motor skill learning, whereas troughs are required for s
187 tex implies that it is a necessary locus for motor skill learning, which we argue is the ability to e
202 ith this mutation, which exhibit deficits in motor-skill learning and abnormal properties of neural c
206 ds abnormal synaptic plasticity and impaired motor-skill learning in mice, and disrupts vocal learnin
208 ns required for sensorimotor integration and motor-skill learning, in particular corticostriatal circ
213 ale older adults show impoverished overnight motor skill memory consolidation relative to young adult
217 or; including hyperactivity, some defects in motor skills, memory impairment, and reduced anxiety, bu
218 es long-term training and the development of motor skills modify the activity of the primary motor co
219 neurological status including fine and gross motor skills, no immune activation and no induction of n
220 rtex of humans identified as responsible for motor skill of the hand (the "knob") was identified in t
224 Candesartan-treated mice also showed better motor skills on the rotarod 3 days after injury, and imp
230 gy (particularly paraesthesia), reduced fine motor skill performance, and worse sensory discriminatio
231 demonstrates that deficits in attention and motor skills persist after 1 year of abstinence from sti
233 activities, which require both motor and non-motor skills, pre-date difficulties in more physically o
234 symptomatic and display loss of language and motor skills, purposeful hand movements, and normal head
236 thesis, such rapid generalization of related motor skills relies on learning the dynamic and kinemati
237 ents, including breathing, walking, and fine motor skills rely on the function of the spinal motor ne
240 tex (M1), after variable practice attenuated motor-skill retention, whereas interference to M1, but n
247 tial learning/memory deficits, impaired fine motor skills, subtly altered social interactions, and de
252 However, successful performance of many motor skills, such as speech articulation, also requires
257 learned in quick succession, declarative and motor skill tasks interfere with one another and subsequ
258 ntifiable degradation in performance on some motor skill tasks supports the need to implement managem
260 rantly conditioned H-reflex change, a simple motor skill that develops gradually and involves plastic
262 smokers, smoking is such a highly practiced motor skill that it often occurs automatically, without
264 corticospinal excitability after learning a motor skill that was subsequently enhanced; whereas, the
265 a different follow-through movement with two motor skills that normally interfere [3-7] allows them t
266 e critical for establishing the rudiments of motor skills that subsequently become refined with furth
267 facilitate simultaneous learning of multiple motor skills that would otherwise interfere with each ot
268 ns of the major descending motor pathway for motor skills, the corticospinal tract (CST), sprout afte
269 by seizures, mental retardation, and loss of motor skills, the first presenting symptom of Batten dis
273 the authors did not observe linkage of hand motor skill to any chromosomal regions implicated in sus
274 icits in attention, verbal memory, and gross motor skills to adulthood schizophrenia-related psychose
275 erformance, and to a lesser degree fine foot motor skills, to a reduction in supraspinal control.
276 ant feeding, and even the assessment of some motor skills too early.We sought to estimate association
277 (LTP) is impaired in the fmr1 KO mouse, and motor skill training does not occlude LTP as it does in
280 r tDCS to modulate its activity during novel motor skill training over the course of 3 d and assessed
281 h Parkinson's disease incorporate goal-based motor skill training to engage cognitive circuitry impor
284 ing shares many characteristics with general motor skills training, thus suggesting several ways of i
285 acilitate learning of at least some types of motor skills using the nonimpaired forelimb as well as s
290 ly 'bogus' oral history of this individual's motor skills was held to have led to 'medical myth makin
292 initial training through mastery of a simple motor skill, we investigate the role of modularity in hu
297 ich differed from those with more adept foot motor skill who activated both the precentral and postce
299 foundation of the co-evolution of linguistic motor skills with the auditory skills underlying speech
300 iatal plasticity during the acquisition of a motor skill, with most neurons in mutants showing negati
WebLSDに未収録の専門用語(用法)は "新規対訳" から投稿できます。