コーパス検索結果 (1語後でソート)
通し番号をクリックするとPubMedの該当ページを表示します
1 nts, and insects, may arise through a simple Pavlovian ability to integrate two learned associations.
2 This complex form of conditioning involves pavlovian and instrumental components, which produce com
4 showed that motivational biases reflect both Pavlovian and instrumental effects: reward and punishmen
5 new insights into contributions of not only Pavlovian and instrumental learning but also habit learn
6 at contextual drug-memory reconsolidation in Pavlovian and instrumental settings involves distinct ne
7 ould be apparent only in the presence of the pavlovian and instrumental stimuli that underwent freque
9 tivity in reward prediction functions during Pavlovian and operant conditioning tasks, less is unders
10 evaluation performance when switched between Pavlovian and operant devaluation tasks, but not when sw
13 In addition, modeling the acquisition of Pavlovian and spatial conditioning tasks has suggested t
16 kg) on the likelihood and duration of a cued Pavlovian approach and a cued operant lever-press respon
17 of trials in which animals executed both the Pavlovian approach and operant lever-press, while raclop
18 d instrumental response and faster to reduce Pavlovian approach behavior under an omission schedule.
19 effect could be better captured as increased Pavlovian approach in an approach-avoidance decision mod
20 Here, we investigated a potential role for Pavlovian approach in biasing which information humans w
21 s to determine how individual differences in Pavlovian approach responses are represented in neural f
22 valence of all three biases was related to a Pavlovian approach-avoid parameter quantified within an
26 tors, most strikingly by biases arising from Pavlovian associations that facilitate action in pursuit
27 re-encounter by combining previously learned Pavlovian associations with novel physiological informat
29 oidance computational model, we found that a Pavlovian attraction to potential reward declined with a
30 Here, using rats with fully consolidated pavlovian auditory fear memories, we demonstrate a doubl
31 cess in which fear is first acquired through Pavlovian aversive conditioning (so-called fear conditio
35 ce, in accordance with theories that ascribe Pavlovian behavioural inhibition, via serotonin, a role
36 meters reflective of the latent influence of Pavlovian bias and how it was modulated by midfrontal th
38 of ability to overcome the influence of the Pavlovian bias, and this effect was most pronounced in s
39 arning model that characterizes a prepotent (pavlovian) bias to withhold responding in the face of ne
41 non-selective increase in the expression of Pavlovian biases; or that stress, as an aversive state,
43 ued reward representation was used to modify Pavlovian conditional goal-approach responses according
45 tly in STs and GTs, as indicated by tests of Pavlovian conditioned approach and conditioned reinforce
46 ell)), enhances selectively a unique form of pavlovian conditioned approach and mediates D1R-dependen
47 ntenance, extinction, and reacquisition of a Pavlovian conditioned approach procedure in adult rats w
49 king behavior if treated systemically before pavlovian conditioned approach training with the CHT inh
50 ntal cortex (antOFC) on anxious behavior and Pavlovian conditioned autonomic and behavioral fear resp
51 (neuroanatomical subdivisions) in processing Pavlovian conditioned fear has been studied extensively,
52 These results are the first to demonstrate Pavlovian conditioned inhibition in SHRs and to use summ
53 xperiments in rats, variation in the form of Pavlovian conditioned responses (CRs) and associated dop
54 ne exposure enhances behavioral responses to pavlovian conditioned stimuli by amplifying accumbal res
55 such as amphetamine enhances the effects of pavlovian conditioned stimuli on conditioned behavior.
56 ing for a reinforcer when in the presence of Pavlovian conditioned stimuli that were separately paire
57 he environmental context in which a discrete Pavlovian conditioned stimulus (CS) is experienced can p
59 ehaviors that reduce the fear aroused by the Pavlovian conditioned stimulus are reinforced through in
60 lished the general excitatory influence of a Pavlovian conditioned stimulus on instrumental performan
63 s previously reported), but also in updating Pavlovian-conditioned responses to morphine-associated s
64 ug seeking, the influence of drug-associated Pavlovian-conditioned stimuli on drug seeking and relaps
66 ral reinforcement learning contexts, such as Pavlovian conditioning and decisions guided by reward hi
68 In this study, human participants underwent Pavlovian conditioning and extinction before we manipula
69 of membrane DOR expression in CINs and both pavlovian conditioning and pavlovian-instrumental transf
70 e projections from the VTA are necessary for Pavlovian conditioning and specifically implicate the PF
71 ygdala (LA) during consolidation of aversive pavlovian conditioning and that this memory requires cap
73 rogress in understanding the neural basis of Pavlovian conditioning has stimulated a new wave of rese
74 hat the interaction between instrumental and Pavlovian conditioning induces powerful motivational bia
97 ss tasks that examine food-specific satiety, Pavlovian conditioning, reinforcer devaluation, and simu
105 omotes a negative reward prediction error in Pavlovian conditioning.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Stimuli th
106 ing in dopamine neurons was not required for Pavlovian conditioning; however, NMDARs in D(1) dopamine
107 N=25, aged 19-52 years) completed a passive (Pavlovian) conditioning task with appetitive (monetary g
108 n subjects, those who performed better under Pavlovian conflict exhibited higher midfrontal theta pow
110 gated the involvement of these structures in pavlovian contingency learning using a task in which the
111 ts cast light on the underlying structure of Pavlovian control and argue that generally this influenc
114 xt A) or a novel context (context B) using a Pavlovian cue extinction procedure designed to mimic hum
116 ng behavior during the previously inhibitory Pavlovian cue than adults, indicative of greater behavio
117 ng adrenergic signaling on the strength of a Pavlovian cue-alcohol memory, using a behavioral procedu
118 ch a target for disrupting well-consolidated pavlovian cue-drug memories following an extensive drug
121 uired for memory formation, is necessary for Pavlovian cued fear conditioning, whether it is downstre
124 demonstrate in rats that dopamine evoked by Pavlovian cues increases during acquisition, but dissoci
125 attenuated the inhibiting effect of aversive Pavlovian cues on instrumental behavior, while leaving u
126 nd that MD damage disrupted the influence of pavlovian cues over action selection but left intact rat
127 erent liquid food rewards in the presence of pavlovian cues previously associated with one of these o
131 rons are crucial for appetitive responses to Pavlovian cues, including cue-induced reinstatement of d
134 tion of a learned cue contradicts views that Pavlovian desires are essentially based on previously le
135 uation tasks, but not when switched from one Pavlovian devaluation task to another Pavlovian devaluat
141 to exhibit greater relapse vulnerability to Pavlovian drug cues paired with drug delivery, here, we
144 ial agonist d-cycloserine administered after pavlovian extinction of cocaine cues in the nucleus accu
146 the essential cerebellar brain circuits for Pavlovian eyeblink conditioning appeared relatively comp
154 AP044 reduces freezing during acquisition of Pavlovian fear and reduces innate anxiety, which is cons
155 ely deleting A2ARs in the striatum increased Pavlovian fear conditioning (both context and tone) in s
159 ies in associative memory processes, such as Pavlovian fear conditioning and extinction, have been ob
169 r losartan treatment, we performed classical Pavlovian fear conditioning pairing auditory cues with f
172 obiological models of fear stem largely from Pavlovian fear conditioning studies that focus on how a
174 of fear have been studied extensively using Pavlovian fear conditioning, a procedure that allows exp
175 study the effect of a sleep phase on rodent Pavlovian fear conditioning, a task with both hippocampu
176 upt the consolidation and reconsolidation of Pavlovian fear conditioning, a widely studied rodent mod
178 ypes can be modeled in animal subjects using Pavlovian fear conditioning, allowing investigation of t
181 t inactivation of striatal A2ARs facilitates Pavlovian fear conditioning, while inactivation of extra
190 results reveal that S-S associations support Pavlovian fear memories after overtraining in both intac
191 s not known whether the associative basis of Pavlovian fear memory acquired by rats with BLA lesions
192 s the consolidation and reconsolidation of a Pavlovian fear memory and associated neural plasticity i
193 g the consolidation and reconsolidation of a Pavlovian fear memory, a widely studied animal model of
195 consolidation' of a recently formed auditory Pavlovian fear memory; fear memory retrieval (reactivati
196 d impairs the 'consolidation' of an auditory Pavlovian fear memory; short-term memory (STM) is intact
198 FC safety signaling, we used neuroimaging of Pavlovian fear reversal, a paradigm that involves flexib
200 cause cues associated with drugs can acquire Pavlovian incentive motivational properties, and acting
201 he neural mechanisms by which basic negative Pavlovian influences guide decision-making during planni
202 is recomputed at reencounter by integrating Pavlovian information with the current brain/physiologic
203 st that serotonin is selectively involved in Pavlovian inhibition due to aversive expectations and ha
208 gate in humans the role of serotonin in such Pavlovian-instrumental transfer in both the aversive and
210 menon is studied in the laboratory using the pavlovian-instrumental transfer protocol in which a stim
211 in CINs and both pavlovian conditioning and pavlovian-instrumental transfer provides a highly specif
216 on response invigoration and action bias in Pavlovian-instrumental transfer, a test of cue-elicited
217 late the performance of instrumental action (Pavlovian-instrumental transfer, PIT), serve as conditio
220 hypofunction, decreasing MD activity during Pavlovian learning impaired the ability of conditioned s
225 onversely, AC5KO mice showed intact aversive pavlovian learning, suggesting the deficit was specific
226 procedure that facilitates the extinction of Pavlovian learning, there was no evidence in any experim
227 jumped and gnawed on the suddenly attractive Pavlovian lever cue, despite never having tasted intense
228 nce in rats when BLA lesions were made after pavlovian light-food pairings but before devaluation by
229 ifically impact action production due to the Pavlovian linkage between inaction and aversive states.
231 nt cues promoted generalized (in)action in a Pavlovian manner, whereas outcomes enhanced instrumental
232 pting the reconsolidation of the maladaptive Pavlovian memories that can precipitate relapse to drug-
233 the specific contribution of one maladaptive Pavlovian memory to relapse, the acquisition of a new al
237 evaluation performance in rats, using either pavlovian or instrumental training procedures and either
239 ion task, varying the type of contingencies (Pavlovian or operant), the number of reinforcers (one vs
240 ol in which a stimulus predicting a specific pavlovian outcome biases choice toward those actions ear
241 l review evidence, from our own work using a Pavlovian over-expectation task as well as from other so
242 the proposed linkage, showing in a modified Pavlovian over-expectation task that brief pauses in the
248 ls do not contribute to the opioid-dependent Pavlovian overshadowing obtained in our preparation.
256 neurons in monkeys (Macaca mulatta) during a Pavlovian procedure with appetitive and aversive outcome
257 l mechanisms, we conditioned monkeys using a Pavlovian procedure with two distinct contexts: one in w
258 y) a rewarding or punishing outcome during a Pavlovian procedure, or unexpectedly received an outcome
260 ortcut, namely aversive pruning, a reflexive Pavlovian process that involves neglecting parts of the
261 neural signatures of an important reflexive Pavlovian process that shapes goal-directed evaluations
262 review behavioral studies that suggest that Pavlovian processes can exert an important influence ove
263 ceives dopaminergic input and is involved in Pavlovian processes that influence choice behavior.
268 AA produces an ilPFC-mediated diminution of pavlovian reactions that extends beyond the training con
271 which we assessed extinction of the learned Pavlovian response, first by pairing two cues together i
272 m and studied the acquisition of conditioned Pavlovian responses using combined physiological recordi
274 receptors mediates action selection based on Pavlovian reward expectations, but is not critical for f
279 bitofrontal cortex (OFC) of monkeys during a Pavlovian task in which the relative amount of liquid re
281 Similarly, threat responses acquired during Pavlovian threat conditioning often return after extinct
282 retroactively strengthen episodic memory for Pavlovian threat-conditioned events, but that, in contra
289 tor system in cue-motivated behavior using a Pavlovian-to-instrumental transfer task designed to asse
290 leus accumbens core of rats (n = 9) during a Pavlovian-to-instrumental transfer task in which the eff
294 dorsal hippocampus (DHPC) in acquisition of Pavlovian trace conditioning and interval timing was exa
297 er maze, T-maze spontaneous alternation, and pavlovian trace fear conditioning), mutants were impaire
298 hange in reward in rats during training in a Pavlovian unblocking task, finding more cells responding
299 ive conditioning paradigm in which different Pavlovian visual cues probabilistically predicted therma
WebLSDに未収録の専門用語(用法)は "新規対訳" から投稿できます。