コーパス検索結果 (1語後でソート)
通し番号をクリックするとPubMedの該当ページを表示します
1 ous processes are as likely to be selfish as prosocial.
2 amic networks only promote cooperation among prosocials.
3 social deficits were seen in the emission of prosocial 50-kHz ultrasonic vocalizations (USV) parallel
4 asticity and cognition, but led to increased prosocial 50-kHz USV emission rates and enhanced social
7 ncies; and (c) the macro level--the study of prosocial actions that occur within the context of group
9 ild on this study by introducing intentional prosocial activities into classrooms and recommending th
12 atic memories within the context of positive/prosocial affects can facilitate diverse psychotherapies
13 es children's sleep, school performance, and prosocial and aggressive behaviors and that these effect
14 hile children watched characters engaging in prosocial and antisocial actions in two different tasks.
15 manization, emphasizing its implications for prosocial and antisocial behavior and for moral judgment
16 sponses as sympathy and personal distress to prosocial and antisocial behavior are discussed, as is t
19 cial to mental health due to its anxiolytic, prosocial and antistress effects, but evidence for anxio
21 relationships comprised of a balance of both prosocial and conflicted interactions create experiences
22 merging biases might help explain the robust prosocial and financial biases men exhibit toward attrac
23 and maternal-perinatal association, modulate prosocial and inbreeding-avoidance behaviors toward spec
24 che also coincides with the emergence of the prosocial and neurobehavioral skills of middle childhood
27 is no difference in reports of anger between prosocial and selfish individuals after finding out that
28 lassify participants a priori as egoistic or prosocial and then embedded them in homogeneous networks
29 use more energy from the communal resource, prosocials are less likely to act on their anger and ret
33 ggression (P=.03) and an increase in neutral/prosocial behavior (P=.04) in the intervention group com
35 its capacity for widespread cooperation and prosocial behavior among large and genetically heterogen
37 e.g., common group identity) can account for prosocial behavior and cooperative norms without the nee
38 n motives form a powerful force in promoting prosocial behavior and enabling large-scale cooperation
39 ship between neurochemical systems and human prosocial behavior and have potential implications for o
42 ognitive perspective taking as precursors of prosocial behavior and suggest that these discrete route
46 unique evidence that serotonin could promote prosocial behavior by enhancing harm aversion, a prosoci
47 rative breeding facilitates the emergence of prosocial behavior by presenting cottontop tamarins (Sag
50 ncreased attentiveness to others and greater prosocial behavior compared to individuals of higher SES
52 ts to others was personally costly, rates of prosocial behavior dropped across all six societies as c
54 e that experience of conspecific companions' prosocial behavior facilitates prosocial behavior in chi
56 While a substantial body of scholarship on prosocial behavior has provided evidence of such norms,
59 ted whether direct reciprocity could promote prosocial behavior in brown capuchin monkeys (Cebus apel
62 ence for an empirically identifiable form of prosocial behavior in humans, which we call "strong reci
65 nary explanations also account for increased prosocial behavior in situations in which attractive ind
66 amework could provide insights into atypical prosocial behavior in those with disorders of social cog
67 ocial males in brain regions associated with prosocial behavior including the bed nucleus of the stri
69 frameworks model the conditions under which prosocial behavior is evolutionarily viable, yet no unif
73 t observations have suggested that, instead, prosocial behavior may reflect an intrinsic value placed
74 It is unclear, however, whether OT promotes prosocial behavior per se, or whether it facilitates soc
75 source of human altruism by suggesting that prosocial behavior results, in part, from our broader te
77 These results demonstrate that OXT promotes prosocial behavior through direct effects on VTA DA neur
80 wing breaches of trust, oxytocin facilitates prosocial behavior while modulating neural signals in th
81 , population density) with psychology (e.g., prosocial behavior), (b) process studies that clarify wh
84 erienced low status showed more communal and prosocial behavior, and endorsed more egalitarian life g
85 review work exploring responses to inequity, prosocial behavior, and other relevant behaviors in nonh
86 Ralpha are necessary for high levels of male prosocial behavior, and provide the first direct evidenc
87 ate that group attachment positively affects prosocial behavior, and that this effect is not simply t
88 sion of public goods, collective action, and prosocial behavior, and we give special attention to fie
90 ligion contours people's moral judgments and prosocial behavior, the relation between religiosity and
91 rewards for the donor for a stricter test of prosocial behavior, while reducing separation distress a
106 We identify three levels of analysis of prosocial behavior: (a) the "meso" level--the study of h
107 ave generally positive effects on normative 'prosocial' behavior, recent laboratory research suggests
108 Therefore, this commentary can show why prosocial behaviors are biased toward physically attract
112 es live in highly social environments, where prosocial behaviors promote social bonds and cohesion an
113 n indicates that testosterone can also cause prosocial behaviors that are appropriate for increasing
114 length) and, second, the different types of prosocial behaviors that exist in social interactions.
116 hypothesized to be a critical facilitator of prosocial behaviors, but the link between empathy and pr
124 es that oxytocin mainly enhances affiliative prosocial behaviors; the fear/stress theory suggests tha
126 erformance suggested a reduced expression of prosocial behaviour, associated with decreased grey matt
127 th increased risk of suboptimum outcomes for prosocial behaviour, fine motor, communication, and soci
130 omists, attractiveness-related financial and prosocial biases are the result of preferences or prejud
131 rrent perspectives on attractiveness-related prosocial biases emphasize the contribution of evolution
133 more important role in driving financial and prosocial biases toward attractive adults than previousl
134 provide an important service in highlighting prosocial biases toward attractive people from a cross-d
135 people are causally related to financial or prosocial biases toward them is weak or nonexistent.
136 There are numerous behaviours that appear prosocial but, on scrutiny, may not have been intended a
137 to the suggestion that well-being and other prosocial characteristics might be enhanced through trai
138 ferences predicted children's preference for prosocial characters and were influenced by parental val
140 hs) would engage in direct reciprocity in a 'prosocial choice test' where a donor could select either
141 al captive studies and in the wild, thus far Prosocial Choice Tests have failed to produce evidence.
143 between selfish and generous motives during prosocial choice, consistent with ideas that the TPJ pro
145 aling OT, monkeys increased the frequency of prosocial choices associated with reward to another monk
148 ship between dopaminergic mechanisms and two prosocial concerns at the core of a number of widely use
152 te the discriminant validity between similar prosocial constructs and highlight the different prosoci
153 how phenomena such as altruistic punishment, prosocial contagion, self-other similarity, and numerous
154 drives learning only when we are acting in a prosocial context and signals a prosocial prediction err
158 ther neural sensitivity to eudaimonic (e.g., prosocial decisions) and hedonic (e.g., selfish rewards
159 l overlooks the contribution of a relational/prosocial dimension to the enjoyment of negative emotion
160 the precise boundaries of this surprisingly prosocial disposition has implications for understanding
161 ether young children increase their level of prosocial donating in response to an upwards shift in ge
163 derscoring a fear-reducing and strategically prosocial effect of testosterone on human social behavio
165 ltural variants were then selected for their prosocial effects in a long-term, cultural evolutionary
166 molecules of the decade due to its profound prosocial effects in nonvertebrate and vertebrate specie
167 peptide oxytocin (OXT) exerts anxiolytic and prosocial effects in the central nervous system of roden
170 hat suppression of "fear" could underlie the prosocial effects of amygdala manipulations, our data st
171 e welcome Norenzayan et al.'s claim that the prosocial effects of beliefs in supernatural agents exte
174 g the neurochemical mechanisms mediating the prosocial effects of MDMA could help in the development
176 opose that religious beliefs with incidental prosocial effects propagated via a long-term process of
177 ocessing, moral reasoning, and processing of prosocial emotions such as guilt and embarrassment may c
180 , once cooperation is prevalent, we show how prosocial genes favoring cooperation and punishment may
182 ealed that intranasal administration of the "prosocial" hormone oxytocin (OT) activates the frontal c
183 d choose between two tokens, with one being "prosocial" in that it rewarded both individuals (i.e., 1
184 useful to quantify individual differences in prosocial inclination that are not influenced by concern
185 ing interpersonal violations (Studies 1, 2), prosocial intentions (Study 3), and economic exploitatio
186 lated past event of helping others increased prosocial intentions to help the present person in need,
192 icacy (e.g., passing bills), suggesting that prosocial language has an independent, direct effect on
194 1996 and 2014 found that declining levels of prosocial language strongly predicted public disapproval
196 y in the neural and behavioral efficiency of prosocial learning, which is predicted by trait empathy.
199 the past oxytocin was conceived merely as a prosocial molecule that nonselectively facilitated affil
201 orical cultures that were not concerned with prosocial morality or with public statement of belief.
202 A multifaceted construct, empathy includes a prosocial motivation or intention to help others in need
203 irming previous findings suggesting a common prosocial motivation underlying altruism and cooperation
205 ocial constructs and highlight the different prosocial motivations underlying economic game behaviour
208 reasingly cooperative, altruistic, and other prosocial norms of interaction from exploitation, especi
209 were spontaneously prosocial, selecting the prosocial option at the same rate regardless of whether
211 ly, this was the case even when choosing the prosocial option was materially costly for the subject (
217 ulatory roles of anger and guilt, as well as prosocial or selfish social preferences in a repeated so
218 embedded them in homogeneous networks of all prosocials or all egoists, or in heterogeneous networks
220 tra-technical proficiency constructs such as prosocial organizational behavior, and overall job perfo
223 c achievement and engagement, antisocial and prosocial peer affiliations, mother-child and father-chi
224 le a predictive division between proself and prosocial people and proves that people have attitude to
226 acting in a prosocial context and signals a prosocial prediction error conforming to classical princ
227 hat both partners alternated making choices, prosocial preference significantly increased, leading to
229 results contradict the suggested role of the prosocial preferences hypothesis and show how the comple
230 breeding common marmosets, and indicate that prosocial preferences in a food donation task do not eme
233 ecome an accepted paradigm that humans have "prosocial preferences" that lead to higher levels of coo
234 when nodes are occupied by persons with more prosocial preferences, who tend to attract and keep more
235 rely different sub-populations is confirmed: prosocial punishers on the one hand, who behave fairly a
236 Resulting from their greater cooperation, prosocials' relations are more stable, yielding substant
238 ltural evolutionary theory of the origins of prosocial religions and apply it to resolve two puzzles
242 iscuss Abrahamic religions as the best-known prosocial religions, but the evidence shows that the cas
245 ultural group selection as an explanation of prosocial religiosity, we propose an alternative that vi
248 ial acts did not require personal sacrifice, prosocial responses increased steadily as children matur
249 nts that view maternal behaviour as tuned to prosocial responsiveness, by showing that vital elements
250 reward funds facilitates the maintenance of prosocial rewarding but prevents its invasion, and that
254 er, the evolutionary foundation of the human prosocial sentiment remains poorly understood, largely b
255 ocial behavior by enhancing harm aversion, a prosocial sentiment that directly affects both moral jud
256 esigned to examine the phylogenetic range of prosocial sentiments and behavior is beginning to shed s
259 the micro level--the study of the origins of prosocial tendencies and the sources of variation in the
260 ecent commentators have suggested that these prosocial tendencies arise from our unique capacity to u
261 xamining the cognitive mechanisms underlying prosocial tendencies has focused on the facilitating rol
267 tanding, and trust), which lead generally to prosocial thoughts and behaviors, or attempts to fortify
268 ard for the actor only (1/0), and the other "prosocial" token rewarding both the actor and a partner
271 ent and recompensation games with respect to prosocial traits from the Big Five and HEXACO models of
272 te strong and possibly runaway selection for prosocial traits, without requiring group selection, kin
273 ooperation by dividing people in proself and prosocial types, or appealing to forms of external contr
WebLSDに未収録の専門用語(用法)は "新規対訳" から投稿できます。