戻る
「早戻しボタン」を押すと検索画面に戻ります。

今後説明を表示しない

[OK]

コーパス検索結果 (1語後でソート)

通し番号をクリックするとPubMedの該当ページを表示します
1 t not dorsal, striatum induced a decrease in risk taking.
2 ne self-administration causes alterations in risk taking.
3 t that may help explain seemingly irrational risk taking.
4 ulus correlated significantly with increased risk taking.
5 bility discounting is a measurable aspect of risk taking.
6 ralleling human regulatory focus research on risk taking.
7 ns, aggressive attitudes, and preference for risk taking.
8 he role of the endocrine system in financial risk taking.
9 s before a gambling task increased financial risk taking.
10 ibutes to success in real-world, high-stakes risk taking.
11 lator could lead to the observed decrease in risk taking.
12 modulate longitudinal declines in adolescent risk taking.
13 he PFC disparately contribute to declines in risk taking.
14 illuminate effects of population dynamics on risk-taking.
15  decision-making captured in impulsivity and risk-taking.
16 nd how this relates to trait impulsivity and risk-taking.
17 ng, alcohol and illicit drug use, and sexual risk-taking.
18 nerability marker for mania and pathological risk-taking.
19 dorsal striatum were associated with greater risk-taking.
20  in adulthood that negatively correlate with risk-taking.
21 found no evidence for a consistent change in risk taking across the lifespan [5].
22               Behavioral data showed reduced risk taking after negative feedback (money loss) during
23 ipitation and seasonal dynamics on pesticide risk taking also concentrations below the limit of quant
24 ly exhibit proclivity for reward seeking and risk taking, also has yielded some insight.
25 rstanding factors that are related to sexual risk taking among virologically detectable (VL+) YMSM ca
26            Systemic amphetamine also reduced risk-taking, an effect which was attenuated by D2-like (
27 icate that the relationship between elevated risk taking and cocaine self-administration is bi-direct
28 l and hypothetical monetary rewards modulate risk taking and decision making in the same manner remai
29 ry rewards are widely used as reinforcers in risk taking and decision making studies.
30  hypothetical monetary rewards in studies of risk taking and decision making.
31 ability weighting functions underlying their risk taking and found that participants became more risk
32                                              Risk taking and functional and structural properties of
33 interest is dopamine's influence on economic risk taking and on subjective well-being, a quantity kno
34 ighlighted the relationship between personal risk taking and population risk.
35  of errors during set shifting and increased risk taking and reduced FA in uncinate fasciculus correl
36 red formation of long-term memory, increased risk taking and stimulus seeking behavior, enhanced susc
37 d physiological traits--such as impulsivity, risk taking and stress responsivity--as well as a substa
38 ty to emotional faces may contribute to less risk taking and susceptibility to peer influence in adol
39 vides a possible neural mechanism explaining risk-taking and impaired reinforcement learning in BD.
40 re/exploit trade-off encompasses elements of risk-taking and impulsivity-common behaviors in psychiat
41 (GM), and test whether modernization, sexual risk-taking and parity are associated with greater risk
42 ls of endogenous cortisol predict subsequent risk-taking and price instability.
43 ce were hyperactive and showed more frequent risk-taking and repetitive behaviors, less depression-li
44 examined how modernization influences sexual risk-taking and reproductive health early in demographic
45                                       Sexual risk-taking and reproductive morbidity are common among
46 ongitudinal analysis provides new insight in risk-taking and reward sensitivity in adolescence: (1) c
47 sing effects of modernization on both sexual risk-taking and risk of GM.
48 motor learning, hyperactivity, and increased risk-taking and self-injurious behaviors.
49 -report and testosterone levels), laboratory risk-taking and self-reported risk-taking tendency; and
50 es, a combination suggested to contribute to risk-taking and susceptibility to peer influence during
51                       We find that increased risk-taking and the publication of experimental failures
52 ciated with impaired set shifting, increased risk taking, and impaired integrity of frontolimbic whit
53 lite levels and measures of decision making, risk taking, and impulsivity in smokers and nonsmokers h
54 e was additionally associated with real-life risk taking, and N1 source activity was reduced in visua
55 al predictor of future excessive aggression, risk taking, and premature death among nonhuman primate
56 cuss hypotheses involving "self-medication," risk-taking, and changes in the social climate but concl
57 nsions of emotionality, such as impulsivity, risk-taking, and drug abuse.
58 s characterised by increased impulsivity and risk-taking, and psychological accounts argue that these
59 levels with neurocognitive, decision-making, risk-taking, and self-reported impulsivity measures were
60         Indeed, pathological impulsivity and risk taking are common in patients with serious mental i
61                              The reasons for risk taking are multifactorial and require providers to
62 ievement since traits like aggressiveness or risk-taking are not uniformly positive for success in sc
63 lly problematic substance use showed greater risk taking as well as lower striatal activation relativ
64 bility and loss in BD can potentially modify risk-taking attitudes, which have important public healt
65 se of ART might increase sexual or injecting risk-taking, available research suggests that unprotecte
66 vity Scale) and performance-based behavioral risk taking (Balloon Analogue Risk Task) in bipolar diso
67 -report and testosterone levels), laboratory risk-taking (balloon analog risk task; BART), and self-r
68 e quadratic pattern was found for laboratory risk-taking (BART).
69                          Two perspectives on risk taking became apparent: the procedure-centric persp
70                           This difference in risk taking because of AP medications was not explained
71 real versus hypothetical monetary rewards on risk taking behavior and brain activity, suggesting a ca
72 cence might have more predominant effects on risk-taking behavior (indexed by increases in open-arm a
73 tors of mental health disturbance, including risk-taking behavior (suicidality, self-mutilation, and
74                        We assessed financial risk-taking behavior after 7 consecutive nights of sleep
75 ritical mediator of psychiatric symptoms and risk-taking behavior among sexually abused children.
76 pmental time point associated with increased risk-taking behavior and experimentation with drugs of a
77 ike receptor availability is correlated with risk-taking behavior and sensitivity of prefrontal activ
78 indings suggest dopaminergic augmentation of risk-taking behavior as a potential contributing mechani
79 chievement, and rates of chronic illness and risk-taking behavior at 20 years of age.
80 at right anodal/left cathodal would decrease risk-taking behavior compared with left anodal/right cat
81 opmental period marked by steep increases in risk-taking behavior coupled with dramatic brain changes
82                                    Increased risk-taking behavior has been associated with addiction,
83 models to more fully explore neurobiology of risk-taking behavior in adolescence.
84 novel neural mechanism underlying heightened risk-taking behavior in alcohol-dependent individuals an
85         These findings provide evidence that risk-taking behavior in humans can be directly manipulat
86 , we assessed the role of agonist therapy on risk-taking behavior in PD patients with (n = 22) and wi
87                                              Risk-taking behavior is characterized by pursuit of rewa
88 ear whether subjective value associated with risk-taking behavior is encoded by DA release.
89                        The ability to modify risk-taking behavior may be translated into therapeutic
90 ty correlated with individual differences in risk-taking behavior observed after monetary losses, whe
91 sociated with changes in sexual or injecting risk-taking behavior or diagnosis of sexually transmitte
92 that adolescent alcohol use results in adult risk-taking behavior that positively correlates with pha
93  pharmacological approach to the reversal of risk-taking behavior through normalization of this patte
94 dual differences in pubertal development and risk-taking behavior were contributors to longitudinal c
95                    Individual differences in risk-taking behavior were observed as animals displayed
96 owever, there was no difference in injecting risk-taking behavior with antiretroviral use (OR, 0.90;
97                                 Impulsivity, risk-taking behavior, and elevated stress responsivity a
98     Dopaminergic drugs are known to increase risk-taking behavior, but the underlying mechanisms for
99 terns were reliably predictive of subsequent risk-taking behavior, including a number of regions know
100 r persons with a history of substance abuse, risk-taking behavior, or suicidal ideation.
101 oding of reward outcomes was correlated with risk-taking behavior, with safe-preferring and risk-pref
102 IV prevention efforts by changing individual risk-taking behavior.
103 shown that disruption of the DLPFC increases risk-taking behavior.
104 mine signals in attention-based learning and risk-taking behavior.
105 n dopamine, a neuromodulator associated with risk-taking behavior.
106 e manifestations often include impulsive and risk-taking behavior.
107 me and contributes to adolescents' real-life risk-taking behavior.
108  animals exhibited individual differences in risk-taking behavior; some displayed a preference for th
109                             Several of these risk-taking behaviors constituted a behavioral syndrome
110 the present study was to investigate whether risk-taking behaviors could be decreased using concurren
111 he risk of violence and other antisocial and risk-taking behaviors in adulthood has not been studied
112 isk for serious adverse events by increasing risk-taking behaviors in intoxicated or impaired persons
113  a critical developmental phase during which risk-taking behaviors increase across a variety of speci
114 e propensity of a putative therapy to induce risk-taking behaviors is of interest.
115 ors suggests that populations with boundless risk-taking behaviors leading to negative real-life cons
116        Adolescence has been characterized by risk-taking behaviors that can lead to fatal outcomes.
117 levant to numerous psychiatric disorders and risk-taking behaviors, and different lines of evidence h
118 d ethanol has been associated with increased risk-taking behaviors, harm to adolescent users, impaire
119  abnormalities, including self-injurious and risk-taking behaviors, hyperactivity, and learning and m
120 ameliorated PPI deficits, hyperactivity, and risk-taking behaviors, in a fashion akin to the antipsyc
121                 This leads to characteristic risk-taking behaviors, including noncompliance with medi
122 ts, indoor tanning was associated with other risk-taking behaviors, such as binge drinking (P < .001
123 , abandon medication regimens, and engage in risk-taking behaviors.
124 and adolescents may not be forthcoming about risk-taking behaviors.
125 upraorbital area) was sufficient to decrease risk-taking behaviors.
126 lescent lifestyles or genetically determined risk-taking behaviors.
127 o be associated with increased engagement in risk-taking behaviors.
128 DLPFC by itself did not significantly change risk-taking behaviors; however, when the contralateral D
129 onsistency in, as well as average levels of, risk taking behaviour (i.e. boldness) when individuals w
130  role in financial markets through increased risk taking behaviour, acting via different behavioural
131              ACoA patients demonstrated true risk-taking behaviour as opposed to simple impulsivity.
132 aradigm, patients were found to show genuine risk-taking behaviour with increased deliberation times
133 ing Task to characterize decision-making and risk-taking behaviour, outside of a learning context, in
134         They did, however, exhibit increased risk-taking behaviour, placing higher bets in a measure
135                Similarly, the differences in risk taking between schizophrenia and bipolar disorder w
136  Voxel-based morphometry showed that greater risk-taking bias was also associated with and partially
137                                              Risk-taking bias was assessed by the Cambridge Gamble Ta
138                              With increasing risk-taking bias, the ventral striatum showed decreased
139                             Increased sexual risk taking by individuals using effective HIV preventio
140 ergen thresholds can result in confusion and risk taking by patients with food allergy.
141 ional bias to reward that potentially drives risk taking by priming approach behavior and elevating r
142 dance, reduced quality of life and increased risk-taking by consumers who often ignore PAL.
143  individuals exerted an overriding effect on risk-taking by the entire group, for reasons independent
144                                              Risk taking can be considered normal in adolescents with
145 sistency, independently of average levels of risk-taking, can be advantageous: more consistent indivi
146 tudy to examine how neural activation during risk taking changes over time and contributes to adolesc
147 ssociated with impulsive decision making and risk taking, characteristics that may arise from hyperse
148 ger attacks/aggression, substance abuse, and risk taking compared with women.
149 lesions, OFC lesions significantly decreased risk-taking compared with sham controls, but did not imp
150 ed choice of the large risky reward (greater risk-taking) compared to both prelesion performance and
151                                    Impulsive risk taking contributes to deleterious outcomes among cl
152 onal Personality Questionnaire indicators of risk-taking (d = 0.50) and impulsivity (d = 0.56).
153 on examined whether insula activation during risk-taking decisions-a process shown to be disrupted in
154 ar if this is due to changes in impulsivity, risk taking, deliberation or risk adjustment, and how th
155 e show that this generosity is not caused by risk-taking deviations in nonsocial contexts.
156 nd striatal activation by pump number during risk taking differed with group.
157 o controls suggesting increased activity and risk taking, diminished short term memory, and decreased
158 pant were extracted from the Domain Specific Risk Taking (DOSPERT) scales.
159  determine whether individual differences in risk taking during adolescence predict later propensity
160 rom these experiments indicated that greater risk taking during adolescence predicted greater intake
161  is also a time of increased exploration and risk-taking (e.g., drug use).
162  to better understand the root causes of MSM risk-taking (e.g., poverty, sex work, substance abuse, m
163 PFC, which may contribute to impulsivity and risk-taking exhibited by adolescents [5, 6, 10-12].
164 on impulsivity [F(1,9) = 7.86, P = 0.02] and risk taking [F(1,9) = 9.2, P = 0.01] in the context of g
165                                              Risk-taking has a fourfold pattern varying as a function
166 g emotional expressions, as well as empathy, risk taking, impulsivity, behavior change, and attention
167                              Might increased risk taking in adolescence result in part from underdeve
168 tory and real-world measures of individuals' risk taking in adulthood.
169                  Next, we estimated economic risk taking in daily life using large-scale data from in
170                 Here we demonstrate abnormal risk taking in DYT1 dystonia patients, which is correlat
171                 Studies have shown increased risk taking in healthy individuals after low-frequency r
172 e relationship between dopamine agonists and risk taking in patients with Parkinson's disease with an
173 ationally, such asymmetric learning predicts risk taking in probabilistic tasks.
174 of real and hypothetical monetary rewards on risk taking in the brain.
175          Both groups similarly reduced their risk-taking in high compared to low risk conditions and
176 otherapies for disorders involving excessive risk-taking in humans, such as pathological gambling.
177           Here we assess these influences on risk-taking in patients with pathological behaviors towa
178                                DAA increased risk-taking in PD patients with active ICD symptoms, but
179 ersely associated with trait impulsivity and risk-taking in the bipolar disorder group.
180 paper applies the theory of the evolution of risk-taking in the presence of idiosyncratic and environ
181 ing disorder in obesity with similarities in risk-taking in the reward domain to substance use disord
182 ifferent dopamine receptor subtypes modulate risk-taking in young adult rats, using a "Risky Decision
183 al inefficiency, but not punishment-mediated risk-taking, in adulthood.
184 ses to spatial locations and their levels of risk-taking, indicated by betting, and impulsivity, meas
185                Theory on condition-dependent risk-taking indicates that when prey are in poor conditi
186 consequence effects are strikingly absent in risk-taking individuals.
187 emporal areas dissociated risk-aversive from risk-taking individuals.
188 ing geographically distinct clusters at high risk, taking into account realistic logistical constrain
189 ported ICD is pathological gambling of which risk taking is a prominent feature.
190 k and benefit are inherently subjective, and risk taking is best understood as the interplay between
191                                              Risk taking is central to human activity.
192  addition, the RDT was used to determine how risk taking is modulated by dopamine signaling, particul
193  preparing to engage in one, suggesting that risk taking may be due, in part, to a failure of the con
194                                              Risk taking may be the result of differential maturation
195 se anxiety manipulations; and (ii) decreased risk-taking may be specific to pathological anxiety.
196 ase (ORs of two to three), strong for sexual risk taking, mental ill health, and problematic alcohol
197 is finding suggests that the higher level of risk-taking observed among adolescents may reflect a hig
198 learning allows animals to capitalize on the risk-taking of others, and avoid endangering themselves,
199 nts approach more robust incentives (such as risk taking or drug experimentation) to recruit this cir
200 erences in different choice domains, such as risk taking or saving, and how preferences in different
201            Poor decision making and elevated risk taking, particularly during adolescence, have been
202 g behavior are mild enough to encourage some risk taking, predecision PMC activation by a reward/risk
203           One unanswered question is whether risk-taking preference is associated with striatal rewar
204  and ESR1), irritable temperament (MSRA) and risk-taking propensity (CADM2).
205  is often described as a period of increased risk taking relative to both childhood and adulthood.
206 eater levels of novelty-seeking behavior and risk-taking relative to adults, behaviors associated in
207 ernization is associated with greater sexual risk-taking, report prevalence of gynecological morbidit
208 ulant addiction is often linked to excessive risk taking, sensation seeking, and impulsivity, but in
209 , might not result in substantial changes in risk-taking sexual behaviour by heterosexual couples.
210 ects with binge eating disorder show greater risk-taking, similar to substance-use disorders.
211 n-diabetic patients with high cardiovascular risk, taking statins.
212 g a pharmacological manipulation and a novel risk taking task while performing functional magnetic re
213                        Using an anticipatory risk-taking task (40 BD and 70 healthy volunteers) and a
214 f healthy subjects performing a naturalistic risk-taking task and used a classification analysis appr
215  program during early abstinence completed a risk-taking task during functional magnetic resonance im
216  activation decreased during an experimental risk-taking task over time, with greater declines in VLP
217 k) and the balloon analog risk task (BART; a risk-taking task), and we measured their chronotype in t
218                                       On the risk-taking task, schizophrenia patients were significan
219 n analog risk task; BART), and self-reported risk-taking tendency (Behavior Inhibition System/Behavio
220 ertal hormones and individual differences in risk-taking tendency.
221 s), laboratory risk-taking and self-reported risk-taking tendency; and (2) to test whether individual
222  self-administration in turn caused elevated risk taking that was present following 6 weeks of abstin
223 stimuli in the hungriest animals may reflect risk-taking that can enhance prey capture success.
224 al cortex is thought to influence adolescent risk taking, the specific ways in which it functions are
225 rontal cortex (PFC) may influence adolescent risk taking, the specific ways in which it functions rem
226 le decreased real life measures of financial risk taking through its influence on neuroticism.
227 Specifically, the stress response calibrates risk taking to our circumstances, reducing it in times o
228                    Ex-smokers also had lower risk-taking to rewards compared with non-smokers.
229 hanisms for adaptively adjusting behavioural risk-taking to the current situation.
230 conomically influential class of competitive risk taking-trading in the financial world.
231     Humanity's fossil-fuel use, if unabated, risks taking us, by the middle of the twenty-first centu
232 tal health problems are related to increased risk taking via inconsistent condom use, multiple partne
233 whereas the medial PFC influences adolescent risk taking via its functional neural coupling with rewa
234                           Greater adolescent risk taking was associated with lower striatal D2 recept
235      This effect of unequal distributions on risk taking was driven by upward social comparisons.
236                                              Risk taking was higher in states with greater income ine
237 tion between ADHD symptoms and engagement in risk taking was mediated by perceived benefits.
238 with cathodal tCDS, an important decrease in risk taking was observed.
239 ndicating that lesion-induced alterations in risk-taking were not secondary to changes in appetitive
240 ral PFC are linked to declines in adolescent risk taking, whereas the medial PFC influences adolescen
241 ion of D2-like receptors robustly attenuated risk-taking, whereas drugs acting on D1-like receptors h
242 decreasing the left, might lead to decreased risk taking, which could hold clinical relevance as exce
243 lighted adolescence as a period of increased risk-taking, which is postulated to result from an overa
244 nsular cortex was positively associated with risk-taking, while D2 mRNA expression in orbitofrontal a
245 ivity (FRN) in response to money loss during risk taking with real rewards compared to those with hyp

WebLSDに未収録の専門用語(用法)は "新規対訳" から投稿できます。
 
Page Top