コーパス検索結果 (1語後でソート)
通し番号をクリックするとPubMedの該当ページを表示します
1 ence of category selectivity (e.g., faces or scenes).
2 t with the expected target's size within the scene.
3 icted the same overall coloring as the study scene.
4 based representations of the entire auditory scene.
5 to detect target objects within a background scene.
6 lowly varying, global property of the visual scene.
7 ntially sample different objects in a visual scene.
8 hich are crucial in understanding the visual scene.
9 lone target in a cluttered array or natural scene.
10 Fs that define fine-scale edges in a complex scene.
11 ntribute to the encoding of an entire visual scene.
12 xy for "gist" level, global information in a scene.
13 its encoding different aspects of the visual scene.
14 bstantially alter the visual appearance of a scene.
15 gnals regarding eye movement relative to the scene.
16 n-noise mixtures, here referred to as Global scene.
17 s to encode different features of the visual scene.
18 alternative interpretations of the auditory scene.
19 peech stream rather than the entire auditory scene.
20 ition of percepts to deduce the meaning of a scene.
21 the location of the emotional versus neutral scene.
22 ping retinal images into a consistent visual scene.
23 extract and encode features from the visual scene.
24 speech stream within a multitalker auditory scene.
25 ts and shifts due to movements of the visual scene.
26 f three-dimensional (3D) data from an imaged scene.
27 context, to compute the segmentation of the scene.
28 to the most conspicuous regions in a visual scene.
29 re rarely available for bystander use at the scene.
30 demonstrated using measurements of a dynamic scene.
31 ances (odometry), irrespective of the visual scene.
32 measurements of tungsten light and a static scene.
33 ng the navigational affordances of the local scene.
34 tem based on where a user looks in a virtual scene.
35 which the field scatters from objects in the scene.
36 correct choice was a duplicate of the study scene.
37 two radically different features of a visual scene.
38 s and is trained to store and replay a movie scene.
39 gaze, while monkeys watched video of natural scenes.
40 facial cues embedded in realistic background scenes.
41 be used to predict the affordances of novel scenes.
42 in their ability to visually search complex scenes.
43 transmit information about complex acoustic scenes.
44 us unaware of minor inconsistencies between scenes.
45 gnals under simulated active view of natural scenes.
46 s to actively sample regions of interests in scenes.
47 r fine-scale edge representations of natural scenes.
48 l cortex (PHC) and the processing of spatial scenes.
49 e characteristics of most realistic auditory scenes.
50 mulus categories, such as faces, bodies, and scenes.
51 ow these factors affect detection in natural scenes.
52 lso in physical understanding of objects and scenes.
53 ic conditions or scanned pictures of natural scenes.
54 ther using a diverse set of complex, natural scenes.
55 othness and longitudinal sparsity of natural scenes.
56 egorical targets (cars or people) in natural scenes.
57 ed with increased responses relative to RAND scenes.
58 are being used to link individuals to crime scenes.
59 isual stimuli is a salient feature of visual scenes.
60 namics toward the coding of complex auditory scenes.
61 ocessing of navigationally relevant, complex scenes.
62 ed to successful mnemonic encoding of visual scenes.
63 multitude of objects contained in real-world scenes?
64 select goal-relevant objects from cluttered scenes?
66 s the visual responses in V1 to a stationary scene, 2) that depolarized VIP cells enhance V1 response
68 memory test), participants reported for each scene (32 studied, 64 nonstudied) whether it reminded th
69 memory test), participants reported for each scene (32 studied, 64 nonstudied) whether it was recolle
71 etition between objects in cluttered natural scenes, allowing for the rapid neural representation of
73 e presence of scene context, even though the scenes alone did not evoke category-selective response p
74 or a correct interpretation of the presented scene also correlated significantly with performance in
76 mplex tasks performed by sensory systems is "scene analysis": the interpretation of complex signals a
77 imodal displays of information, (c) auditory scene analysis, (d) enabling and understanding shared au
78 antly by judgments associated with pictorial scene analysis, whereas its anterior section is more act
84 The fibers are randomly distributed in the scene and are packed on the camera end, thus making a br
86 able of multiplexing information of a visual scene and encoding relative depth perception from motion
88 tended speech stream from the whole auditory scene and how increasing background noise corrupts this
89 rates information already acquired about the scene and knowledge of the statistical structure of patt
90 ggests that, although functionally distinct, scene and object processing pathways do interact at a pe
93 r to correctly interpret the PIT 360 degrees scene and tended to significantly focus on details of th
94 amiliarized scene images intermixed with new scenes and classified them as indoor versus outdoor (enc
95 ing pathways.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Although scenes and objects are known to contextually interact in
96 ations of humans freely viewing naturalistic scenes and performing exemplar and categorical search ta
98 the visual system (high-level vision: faces, scenes) and relatively late in visual development (start
99 in activity, a video of the real-life visual scene, and free oculomotor behavior were simultaneously
100 ce and time, functional interactions between scene- and object-processing pathways.SIGNIFICANCE STATE
102 itally blind participants with face-, body-, scene-, and object-related natural sounds and presented
104 dividuals with regard to which features of a scene are fixated [6-8], large individual differences ar
105 NCE STATEMENT Individual objects in a visual scene are seen as distinct entities or as parts of a who
106 ation and recognition of objects in a visual scene are two problems that are hard to solve separately
111 hus, the neural representation of the visual scene as one progresses up the cortical hierarchy become
112 novel therapies for all patients and set the scene as the field prepares to enter an era of novel the
113 could deviate significantly with the object scene, as well as OSH systems with different opical sett
115 involved in fine-grained neural encoding of scenes at a subordinate-level, in our case, architectura
116 ect recognition and neural representation by scene background, even in the absence of contextually as
122 ty quickly tracks the presence of objects in scenes, but crucially only for those objects that were i
124 es a structured representation of the visual scene by co-selecting image elements that are part of be
125 s utilize rapidly acquired information about scenes by guiding search toward likely target sizes.
126 bout target and hand positions from a visual scene, calculates a difference vector between them, and
128 is that barn owls, by active scanning of the scene, can induce adaptation of the tectal circuitry to
132 gnals that inform visual cortex about visual scene changes not predicted by the animal's own actions.
133 erns as early as 160 ms, despite substantial scene clutter and variation in the visual appearance of
135 s, have an attentional bias toward emotional scenes compared with conspecifics showing a neutral expr
137 e and quicker to detect source appearance in scenes comprised of temporally-regular (REG), rather tha
139 nment, we investigate how a complex acoustic scene consisting of multiple speech sources is represent
140 women, we investigate how a complex acoustic scene consisting of multiple speech sources is represent
141 gs suggest that visuospatial integration and scene construction processes might partly mediate indivi
142 psin enhances the thalamic representation of scenes containing local correlations in radiance, compen
144 ly, impacting how both social and non-social scene content are fixated, as well as general visual exp
145 s general tendency for visual exploration of scene content, as well as the precise moment-to-moment s
146 tex was strongly enhanced by the presence of scene context, even though the scenes alone did not evok
147 but easy to recognize within their original scene context, in which no other associated objects were
148 The presence of a specific category in a scene could be reliably decoded from MEG response patter
150 -binned photon detections to highly accurate scene depth and reflectivity by exploiting both the tran
151 nd has revealed selective impairments during scene discrimination following hippocampal lesions.
154 to a single shape class, but to a variety of scene elements that are typically aligned with gravity:
156 y roles in our ability to parse the auditory scene, enabling us to attend to one auditory object or s
157 ir size is inconsistent with the rest of the scene, even when the targets were made larger and more s
159 ires subjects to discriminate highly similar scenes, faces, or objects from multiple viewpoints, and
162 ssion cascade as a framework, and to set the scene for the articles in this series, which address spe
163 ired out-of-equilibrium systems will set the scene for the next generation of molecular materials wit
164 trieval (yes responses) for recently studied scenes for the two test types revealed pronounced differ
165 ate different kinds of boundaries in natural scenes, for example, those arising from surface reflecta
166 acting pattern motion direction from complex scenes--for which decisive evidence for the involvement
169 identifying 20 different objects classes in scenes from a standard computer vision data set (the PAS
170 human subiculum in discrimination of complex scenes from different viewpoints.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT
171 curately infers the speed of complex natural scenes from this set of spatiotemporal channels [6-14].
173 lobal serial dependencies: the appearance of scene gist is sequentially dependent on the gist perceiv
174 ven approach to describe a large database of scenes (>100,000 images) in terms of their visual proper
176 ent response to a sinusoidally moving visual scene has been shown to get smaller with faster stimuli,
177 unts of blood traces can be found at a crime scene, having a method that is nondestructive, and provi
178 spective cohort study of patients undergoing scene HT or ground transport in the National Trauma Data
180 mics, human subjects viewed pre-familiarized scene images intermixed with new scenes and classified t
183 ce area, represents pathways for movement in scenes in a manner that is tolerant to variability in ot
184 we show that variation in viewing of social scenes, including levels of preferential attention and t
186 sults suggest that expectations derived from scene information, processed in scene-selective cortex,
188 when they are inconsistent in size with the scene; instead, it is a byproduct of a useful strategy t
190 The ability to parse a complex auditory scene into perceptual objects is facilitated by a hierar
191 redistribution is perceived as an ancestral scene involving three notional players: the needy other,
192 E STATEMENT Perception of a complex auditory scene is based on the ability of the brain to group thos
194 ntation that contrasts the regularity of the scene is perceived salient for many animals as a means t
196 tical regularities present in noisy acoustic scenes is an important biological strategy for solving c
200 topographical memory as assessed in tasks of scene memory where the viewpoint shifts from study to te
201 tic factors influence gaze to complex visual scenes more broadly, impacting how both social and non-s
202 nst the likely depth structure of the viewed scene, naturally reproducing key characteristics of both
205 we demonstrate that specific regions of the scene network-the retrosplenial complex (RSC) and occipi
207 to track the neural representation of within-scene objects as a function of top-down attentional set.
210 ng technology that can deliver an AED to the scene of an out-of-hospital cardiac arrest for bystander
211 hth century onward, the Indian Ocean was the scene of extensive trade of sub-Saharan African slaves v
212 patients (>/=15 years) transported from the scene of injury to our level I trauma center by air or g
213 tages in the hierarchy transform an auditory scene of multiple overlapping sources, from peripheral t
216 world objects presented in images of natural scenes only when these objects have been associated with
219 Instead of analyzing all the elements in a scene, our visual system has the ability to compress an
220 y by preserving stable aspects of the visual scene over time, yet, for dynamic stimuli, temporal smoo
222 Here, we propose an alternative, that stable scene perception is actively achieved by the visual syst
224 e blindness are usually thought to stabilize scene perception, making us unaware of minor inconsisten
226 is crucial for detection of faces in natural scenes, performing a critical first step on which other
227 ct subfields, examining the role of these in scene processing has been previously limited by scanner
228 ric retinal alignment is required for visual scene recognition, but ants can translate this acquired
229 s delayed when listeners were focused on the scenes relative to when listening passively, consistent
230 This demonstrated that the discrimination of scenes, relative to faces and objects, recruits the ante
231 Finally, the correlation of this effect with scene-selective activity suggests that, although functio
232 esses within PPA, HD patients showed reduced scene-selective activity within PPA compared with health
233 f the STS and extend these prior findings to scene-selective cortex in the ventral-most regions of IT
234 derived from scene information, processed in scene-selective cortex, feed back to shape object repres
236 Multivoxel pattern analyses showed that a scene-selective region of dorsal occipitoparietal cortex
241 found a spatially clustered subpopulation of scene-selective units with an associated event-related f
242 observers often inspect different parts of a scene sequentially to form overall perception, suggestin
248 we show the ventral pathway also represents scene structure aligned with the gravitational reference
250 ct and relay specific features from a visual scene, such as the capacity to discern local or global m
251 rforms deep neural networks on a challenging scene text recognition benchmark while being 300-fold mo
252 population code that is more distributed for scenes than for other stimulus categories, and less spar
254 ifically sustains representations of similar scenes that are less overlapping than in other hippocamp
255 t the DG supports representations of similar scenes that are less overlapping than those in neighbori
257 o distinguish different objects in a natural scene, the brain must segment the image into regions cor
258 Immediately after a change in the visual scene, the motor system generates independent responses
261 hen infants observe an agent act in a simple scene, they infer the agent's mental states and then use
264 128 x 128-pixel resolution three-dimensional scenes to an accuracy of approximately 3 mm at a range o
266 istical regularity present in noisy acoustic scenes to reduce errors in signal recognition and discri
269 s (when road closures are likely) had longer scene-to-hospital transport times than on nonmarathon da
270 ysical inferences per se or, indeed, even in scene understanding; they overlap with the domain-genera
272 tations and processing of a complex acoustic scene up through the hierarchy of the human auditory cor
273 f different sounds in a multitalker auditory scene using magnetoencephalography and corticovocal cohe
274 sities (photons s(-1) mum(-2) ) in a natural scene vary over several orders of magnitude from shady w
276 s, we constructed a novel continuous natural scene video whereby phase information was maintained in
277 orks (DCNNs) and panoramic videos of natural scenes, viewed immersively through a head-mounted displa
278 humans performing a range of everyday tasks (scene viewing and exemplar and categorical search), maki
279 tially to deviant information during natural scene viewing and visual search, and suggest that humans
284 The presence of these categories within a scene was decoded from MEG sensor patterns by training l
289 rsity of environments that humans encounter, scenes were surveyed at random locations within 25 indoo
291 cortex contains a detailed map of the visual scene, which is represented according to multiple stimul
292 s were not evoked by photographs of arousing scenes, which is indicative of selective early reactivit
294 a global representation of the full auditory scene with all its streams is a better candidate neural
295 ed object detector that processes the entire scene with varying resolution, uses retino-specific obje
298 t in the selection of top salient regions of scenes with those selected by a non-foveated high resolu
299 erentially to abstract categories (faces and scenes), with a spatial organization similar to adults.
300 -based representation of the entire auditory scene, with both attended and unattended speech streams
WebLSDに未収録の専門用語(用法)は "新規対訳" から投稿できます。