戻る
「早戻しボタン」を押すと検索画面に戻ります。

今後説明を表示しない

[OK]

コーパス検索結果 (1語後でソート)

通し番号をクリックするとPubMedの該当ページを表示します
1 er to a greater extent than from the foreign speaker.
2  in participants listening to and seeing the speaker.
3 es of five Khoisan individuals and one Bantu speaker.
4 se and elicited approach behavior toward the speaker.
5 ople is the perceived emotional state of the speaker.
6 o track the temporal speech envelope of that speaker.
7 f vowel pronunciation in time and age of the speaker.
8 ational pitch contour, phonetic content, and speaker.
9 ing that are otherwise hidden in monolingual speakers.
10 ntity marker and expressive resource for its speakers.
11 (e.g., a dog bark) in 14 English monolingual speakers.
12  even in the presence of multiple concurrent speakers.
13 ere sequentially presented from two adjacent speakers.
14 ists that was primarily associated with Khoe speakers.
15 d today only among Central American Chibchan-speakers.
16 r systems and by cultural transmission among speakers.
17  likelihood of women appearing as colloquium speakers.
18 ve speakers looked more like those of native speakers.
19 essing biomedical information for non-native speakers.
20 ed in non-native speakers compared to native speakers.
21 igh degree of biological uniformity of their speakers.
22 ing for the gender and rank of the available speakers.
23 s in how linguistic forms are copied between speakers.
24 as then analyzed and compared to that of the speakers.
25 he presence of spatially separated competing speakers.
26 late; Bolivian-Spanish speakers; and English speakers.
27  infants' demonstrated preference for native speakers.
28            The conference hosted 35 renowned speakers, 100 posters, 20 short talks, and a preconferen
29 (33.9%) compared with Spanish (29%) or other speakers (20.4%).
30 estigators, including 55 invited world-class speakers, 25 short oral presenters, and 100 poster prese
31       In experiments 1-5, nonsigning English speakers accurately distinguished between telic (e.g., "
32                                   In speech, speakers adjust their articulatory movement magnitude ac
33             Subjects were 49 healthy English speakers ages 19-62 (30 female).
34 eaker, as if subjects were listening to that speaker alone.
35                      Musicians and Cantonese speakers also showed superior working memory capacity re
36  temporal profiles of brain responses in the speaker and listeners respectively, in turn affecting co
37 isease duration (45 German-Italian bilingual speakers and 40 monolingual speakers) were included.
38 ssembled an amazing forum, which included 53 speakers and 67 poster presentations from laboratories a
39                                          The speakers and audience discussed the special challenges i
40 al Khoesan groups, and highlights that Bantu speakers and Coloured individuals have different mixture
41                              We show (a) how speakers and comprehenders use covert imitation and forw
42  research has shown that the degree to which speakers and listeners exhibit similar brain activity pa
43 rance increases such neural coupling between speakers and listeners.
44 ndings support the notion that tone language speakers and musically trained individuals have higher p
45 s of the non-Khoisan groups, including Bantu-speakers and non-Africans, experienced population declin
46                            All participants (speakers and observers) were monitored via electrocardio
47 Jeanne Nerbonne and Nipavan Chiamvimonvat as speakers and panel discussants.
48 ed the fMRI response time courses of English speakers and Russian speakers who listened to a real-lif
49 the GLN contributes to the visibility of its speakers and the global popularity of the cultural conte
50 ture of the networks connecting multilingual speakers and translated texts, as expressed in book tran
51 onstrated that the success with which single speakers and vowels can be decoded from auditory cortica
52 se temporal dependencies arise between adult speakers' and listeners' neural activity.
53 ons across the (right) mid-anterior STG/STS (speakers) and bilateral mid-posterior STG/STS (vowels),
54 retary, Treasurer, CEO and Medical Director, Speaker, and Speaker-Elect and the chairpersons of the A
55 cretary-Treasurer, CEO and Medical Director, Speaker, and Speaker-Elect and the chairpersons of the A
56 retary, Treasurer, CEO and Medical Director, Speaker, and Speaker-Elect and the chairpersons of the A
57 s, students, engineers, technicians, invited speakers, and guests from North and South America, Germa
58        Half of the participants were English speakers, and half were not.
59 k, multitalker babble was presented from all speakers, and pairs of speech tokens were sequentially p
60 ese social biases, the preference for native speakers, and propose that this preference may result fr
61 en a subject listens to one of two competing speakers, and show that the brainstem response is consis
62 ences are active in the brains of individual speakers, and they are demonstrably distinct from sensor
63 an hunter-gatherer isolate; Bolivian-Spanish speakers; and English speakers.
64 els and organ systems was presented, and the speakers aptly illustrated the unique power of each.
65 e rhythm of a piece of music, the words of a speaker are all examples of temporally structured sensor
66             Personal interest stories of the speakers are discussed, along with the relationships amo
67 ences in speech understanding when competing speakers are present, as in a crowded restaurant.
68                         Using a high-density speaker array, we show that the preferred direction and
69 ears ago, a second expansion of Austronesian-speakers arrived in Near Oceania and the descendants of
70 ish listeners rated pairs of native-language speakers as more dissimilar than foreign-language speake
71 ectral and temporal features of the attended speaker, as if subjects were listening to that speaker a
72 ocessed automatically by the brain of native speakers, as revealed by whole-head electrical recording
73                 A top-notch slate of invited speakers, assembled by conference organizers or committe
74 dy examines gender differences in colloquium speakers at 50 prestigious US colleges and universities
75  The underrepresentation of women as invited speakers at international scientific conferences exempli
76  The perceived underrepresentation of female speakers at prominent scientific meetings is currently a
77                         Here, several of the speakers at that conference answer questions posed by Na
78 sented in ASCB's leadership and as symposium speakers at the annual meeting.
79 ing behavior in these games by assuming that speakers attempt to be informative and that listeners us
80           Comparisons of the demographics of speakers, attendees, and ORGN members are made, and supe
81 (sight, sound, and touch), the location of a speaker (audition and sight), and the rhythm or duration
82 owels /a/, /i/, and /u/) spoken by different speakers (boy, girl, male) and performed a delayed-match
83  speech adapts only to the intensity of that speaker but not to the intensity of the background speak
84 r, (ii) this variability is attested between speakers but not within a speaker, (iii) this variabilit
85   Musicians can perform at different tempos, speakers can control the cadence of their speech, and ch
86 ssifier trained solely on examples of single speakers can decode both attended words and speaker iden
87 the acoustic similarity of both phonetic and speaker categories.
88 erior temporal gyrus/sulcus (STG/STS) during speaker categorization and in the right posterior tempor
89 for Epidemiologic Research hosted 17 invited speakers charged by the Executive Committee with present
90 different word orders are known to influence speakers' choices, but the underlying neural mechanisms
91 witch mechanisms are activated in non-native speakers compared to native speakers.
92                                Behaviorally, speakers compensated for perturbations by changing their
93 general had smaller MMNs compared to English speakers, confirming previous studies demonstrating sens
94 poken object-label mappings across different speaker contexts.
95                                  The keynote speaker, Craig Pikaard, opened the meeting with his pres
96                    To tackle this challenge, speakers described methods for assessing the nutrient de
97                                        Last, speakers described the role of dairy products in global
98    Nine subjects listened to recordings of a speaker describing visual scenes that varied in the degr
99 ir mother tongue better than they do foreign speakers despite their limited speech comprehension abil
100 ers as more dissimilar than foreign-language speakers, despite their inability to understand the mate
101                           While non-Japanese speakers did not change their conversation topics signif
102 ech comprehension abilities, suggesting that speaker discrimination may rely on familiarity with the
103 res of voice perception (vocal size, gender, speaker discrimination) and voice recognition (familiari
104                                              Speakers discussed potential challenges to clinical tran
105 inese and English adult participants to rate speaker dissimilarity in pairs of sentences in English o
106     We conclude that signers gesture just as speakers do.
107 electively listening to one of two competing speakers, either of different or the same sex, using mag
108 urer, CEO and Medical Director, Speaker, and Speaker-Elect and the chairpersons of the APA Committee
109 urer, CEO and Medical Director, Speaker, and Speaker-Elect and the chairpersons of the APA Committee
110 urer, CEO and Medical Director, Speaker, and Speaker-Elect and the chairpersons of the APA Committee
111 hether congruent visual input of an attended speaker enhances cortical selectivity in auditory cortex
112 were more likely than women to be colloquium speakers even after controlling for the gender and rank
113  speech enhances our ability to comprehend a speaker, even in noise-free conditions.
114                      We show that (i) Korean speakers exhibit substantial variability regarding this
115                                      Invited speakers' expertise covered basic as well as translation
116                                          All speakers expressed a sense of urgency about the affordab
117                                              Speaker fees (31.7%), consulting fees (21.6%), and resea
118                          Consulting fees and speaker fees were associated with highest payment amount
119                    We found that early Bantu speakers first moved southward, through the equatorial r
120                                              Speakers focused on translational and clinical applicati
121 A session, followed by a panel discussion of speakers from both sessions.
122 etween Khanty and Bashkir, a group of Turkic speakers from Southern Urals region.
123             Instead, contextual cues such as speaker gaze can provide similar information with a lowe
124                                       Native speakers had enhanced neural processing of the formant f
125 ons in the Pickering & Garrod (P&G) model of speaker-hearer coordination in dialogue.
126                                  The keynote speaker, Ian Willis, opened the meeting with his present
127   The influence of language familiarity upon speaker identification is well established, to such an e
128 tch-to-sample task on either speech sound or speaker identity.
129  speakers can decode both attended words and speaker identity.
130 erent information about phonetic features or speaker identity.
131 s attested between speakers but not within a speaker, (iii) this variability controls interpretation
132 positions for session moderators and invited speakers.IMPORTANCE Politicians and media members have a
133 s excel at selectively listening to a target speaker in background noise such as competing voices.
134 k of four acoustic stimuli via an ultrasonic speaker in random order: (1) 50 kHz USVs, (2) 22 kHz USV
135  selectively to one out of several competing speakers in a "cocktail party" situation is a highly dem
136 to one out of several simultaneously talking speakers in a "cocktail party" situation is a highly dem
137 reek-speaking participants showed that Greek speakers in general had smaller MMNs compared to English
138 issue of Brain Research includes articles by speakers in this meeting and others, which together synt
139                              The work of the speakers in this symposium shows that TEs are expressed
140 were included, allowing the investigation of speaker-independent representations of individual words.
141 xpect to receive information from the native speaker, indicating that infants were preparing to learn
142                 Analysis of the phonetic and speaker information in neural activations revealed that
143 ly, we show a joint encoding of phonetic and speaker information, where the neural representation of
144  listeners use Bayesian inference to recover speakers' intended referents.
145 r is saying, but also, importantly, when the speaker is saying it.
146 l movements relay information about what the speaker is saying, but also, importantly, when the speak
147 s can greatly help in understanding what the speaker is saying.
148 ng of Remote Oceanic islands by Austronesian speakers is a fascinating and yet contentious part of hu
149 dition, the population history of Athapaskan speakers is complex, with the Tlich being distinct from
150 ormation, where the neural representation of speakers is dependent on phoneme category.
151  that most of the vocabulary of the Malagasy speakers is derived from the Barito group of the Austron
152 the intensity of the attended and background speakers is separately varied over an 8-dB range, the ne
153 l pronouns, such as 'I' and 'you', require a speaker/listener to continuously re-map their reciprocal
154 ivation increased with predictability in the speaker, listeners' pSTG activity instead decreased for
155  clear proclivity for male-dominated invited speaker lists.
156  groups from northern Cameroon and non-Bantu speakers living in present-day Nigeria and Ghana.
157 rs were assessed using a restricted range of speaker locations designed to match those found in clini
158 ntly, the networks of more fluent non-native speakers looked more like those of native speakers.
159  effects or drift, or do languages with more speakers lose features due to a process of simplificatio
160 ction and comprehension are interwoven, with speakers making predictions of their own utterances and
161 ly, the results show that lips on the Target speaker matched to a secondary (Mask) speaker's audio se
162 ing analyses of the gender parity of invited speakers may allow the ongoing discussions to be informe
163 s and readers to infer the intended message (speaker meaning) from the coded meaning of the linguisti
164 ce in Island Southeast Asia, or Austronesian speakers migrated to and through the mainland, admixing
165 experience predicted how well native English speakers (N=120) discriminated Norwegian tonal and vowel
166                              Mandarin native speakers named colored line drawings of objects using co
167                              Capitalizing on speakers' natural variability, we contrasted the respons
168 n English syllable in three groups of native speakers, non-native nonmusicians, and non-native musici
169  functions ranging from threat assessment to speaker normalization.
170  contours directly reflected the encoding of speaker-normalized relative pitch but not absolute pitch
171                                          The speakers not only described how metabolic flux adapts to
172 to a state of phonetic equilibrium, in which speakers of all ages share a similar phonetic profile.
173                                              Speakers of all human languages regularly use intonation
174                                           Do speakers of all languages use segmental speech sounds wh
175 using electroencephalography (EEG) in fluent speakers of American Sign Language (ASL) as they watch v
176                              We asked native speakers of Chinese fluent with English to indicate whet
177 across vast geographic distances and between speakers of different language families.
178                                       Slavic speakers of Eastern Europe are, in general, very similar
179 panish-English bilinguals and control native speakers of English in a semantic categorisation task on
180                                   Non-native speakers of English who hold nursing qualifications from
181 at the neural level in native and non-native speakers of English who were overtly naming pictures of
182 ntial (ERP) and tested native and non-native speakers of English.
183 oups, of whom 4 were non-native and 7 native speakers of English.
184                                     However, speakers of Eskimo-Aleut languages from the Arctic inher
185 honemes, but the bulk of evidence comes from speakers of European languages in which the orthographic
186 ntal units of phonological encoding even for speakers of languages that do not encode such units orth
187  could help make English texts accessible to speakers of other languages.
188 erage genomes for 83 Aboriginal Australians (speakers of Pama-Nyungan languages) and 25 Papuans from
189 alKhomani Bushmen of South Africa, including speakers of the nearly extinct N|u language.
190 etreat of a language is the interaction with speakers of the same language.
191       However, 7-mo-old infants discriminate speakers of their mother tongue better than they do fore
192 nce from nuclear and mtDNA loci suggest that speakers of these language families share a distinct bio
193 ly represented, raising the possibility that speakers of these languages do not use phonemes as funda
194  varies unpredictably across a population of speakers of what is ostensibly a single language.
195 hat people do not always mean what they say; speakers often use imprecise, exaggerated, or otherwise
196             A major exception is in Chibchan speakers on both sides of the Panama isthmus, who have a
197 sicians to that of tone-language (Cantonese) speakers on tasks of auditory pitch acuity, music percep
198 ons for graphene, such as remote non-contact speakers, optical-switching acoustic devices, etc.
199  then retrieval/reexposure effects on either speaker or listener, with a focus on retrieval-induced f
200 ct with to a much higher degree than English speakers or the global average.
201      We compiled the full rosters of invited speakers over the last 35 years for four prominent inter
202 brought together consortium members, outside speakers, patient advocacy groups, and young investigato
203     We continuously presented native Chinese speakers peripherally with Chinese homophone characters
204                     Do languages with larger speaker populations change faster due to a greater capac
205 ns ago, among upper castes and Indo-European speakers predominantly.
206 to reviewing existing evidence in the field, speakers presented their own original research to provid
207 mprehension/production explains intra-/inter-speaker priming.
208 eversal of the well-established finding that speakers produce less sophisticated language than they c
209                    We find that the gestures speakers produce when they talk are integral to communic
210 y that was not present in the English native speakers, raising the possibility that additional attent
211 es shifting the pitch of the feedback that a speaker receives, known as pitch-shifted feedback.
212                            Finally, the WICB Speaker Referral Service provides a list of outstanding
213 nds in which the salience of intonational or speaker-related (suprasegmental) vocal cues was increase
214 h segmental phonemic cues and suprasegmental speaker-related and emotional prosodic cues.
215 mpared a "same sentence" condition, in which speakers repeated the study utterances at test, and a "d
216 ased on cortical responses to the mixture of speakers reveal the salient spectral and temporal featur
217  absent in Eskimo-Aleut and northern Na-Dene speakers, revealed that this haplogroup arose in North A
218 s and licenses (1 of 13 [7.7%]), and faculty/speaker roles (2 of 48 [4.2%]).
219 s and licenses (1 of 22 [4.6%]), and faculty/speaker roles (21 of 189 [11.1%]) in 2014.
220 ch the neural activity is coupled across the speaker's and listener's brains during production and co
221 Target speaker matched to a secondary (Mask) speaker's audio severely increase the participants' comp
222  was significantly more synchronous with the speaker's brain activity for highly predictive contexts
223 of the following: stock ownership, employee, speaker's bureau, and consultant).
224                                     Seeing a speaker's face as he or she talks can greatly help in un
225           The behavioral benefit of seeing a speaker's face during conversation is especially pronoun
226                                     Seeing a speaker's face enhances speech intelligibility in advers
227  response to speech, we found that viewing a speaker's face enhances the capacity of auditory cortex
228 e behavioral benefit arising from seeing the speaker's face was not predicted by changes in local enc
229 peech comprehension is improved by viewing a speaker's face, especially in adverse hearing conditions
230                          This is because the speaker's facial movements relay information about what
231 rence between oscillatory brain activity and speaker's lip movements and demonstrated significant ent
232 ce between activity in visual cortex and the speaker's lips.
233 y predicted when they were less similar to a speaker's median production, even though the prediction
234    You can form a good idea of the different speaker's mood and affective state, as well as more subt
235                        We also explore how a speaker's motive to form a shared reality with listeners
236 e of intonation contours independent of each speaker's pitch range.
237  and either inferred the authenticity of the speaker's state, or judged how much laughs were contagio
238 s in cardiac activity that were based on the speaker's stress level.
239 hesis that a listener's ability to predict a speaker's utterance increases such neural coupling betwe
240 s a remarkable ability to attend to a single speaker's voice in a multi-talker background.
241 ticipants and the presence of a woman on the speaker selection committee correlated with improved par
242 a suggest that those who invite and schedule speakers serve as gender gatekeepers with the power to c
243                   Interestingly Greek native speakers showed some P3a activity that was not present i
244                        Compared with English speakers, Spanish speakers were less likely to have high
245                 We next show that sign-naive speakers spontaneously project these principles to novel
246 r but not to the intensity of the background speaker, suggesting an object-level intensity gain contr
247 quencies did not differ from those of native speakers, suggesting that musical training may compensat
248                                         When speakers talk, they gesture.
249 that are reliably activated in the brains of speakers telling a 15-min-long narrative.
250 oscopy (fNIRS) to record brain activity of 3 speakers telling stories and 15 listeners comprehending
251                  We argue that evidence from speakers' tendency to repeat their own and others' struc
252 supported by finding that, within non-native speakers, there was less auditory feedback for those wit
253                         (a) Gesture reflects speakers' thoughts, often their unspoken thoughts, and t
254                       (b) Gesture can change speakers' thoughts.
255 eparing to learn information from the native speaker to a greater extent than from the foreign speake
256 e created a "phantom road" using an array of speakers to apply traffic noise to a roadless landscape,
257 al in the online control of speech, allowing speakers to compare their self-produced speech signal wi
258 etoencephalographic imaging (MEG-I) in human speakers to demonstrate that efference copy prediction d
259                                  Encouraging speakers to gesture can thus provide another route for t
260  idea, but it is not clear what it means for speakers to predict their own utterances, and how predic
261 ontribution of western central African Bantu speakers to the ancestry of African Americans, whose gen
262 ng more like Yeniseian, Ugric, and Samoyedic speakers to the north, and southern Altaians having grea
263 on, here we gauge the sensitivity of English speakers to the putative universal syllable hierarchy (e
264             Proficient language use requires speakers to vary word order and choose between different
265 n of possible instances, or tokens, in which speakers use AAVE rather than SAE speech features.
266 subjects watched and listened to videos of a speaker uttering consonant vowel (CV) syllables /ba/ and
267 tude growth were calculated for an ear canal speaker versus the intracochlear actuator for tone burst
268                       The task dependency of speaker/vowel classification demonstrates that the infor
269                                              Speaker/vowel classification relied on distinct but over
270 gs selected by a music therapist via ambient speakers) vs standard care.
271 ologists that attended the meeting, choosing speakers was a unique challenge.
272  also found that genetic adaptation of Bantu speakers was facilitated by admixture with local populat
273 The difference between native and non-native speakers was further supported by finding that, within n
274 ed in a listening task with two simultaneous speakers, we demonstrate that population responses in no
275  an Information Masking Task with concurrent speakers, we find significantly more errors in the decis
276 , whereas the odds that it rushed toward the speaker were more than sixfold higher.
277 ch word, pronunciations from three different speakers were included, allowing the investigation of sp
278      Compared with English speakers, Spanish speakers were less likely to have high scores in pizza a
279                                        Here, speakers were separated by 15 degrees around the midline
280 talian bilingual speakers and 40 monolingual speakers) were included.
281 ormal healthy human subjects (native English speakers) were scanned while they listened to 10 consona
282 ients were excluded if they were not English speakers, were not prescribed treatment for their acne,
283 moting preferential tracking of the attended speaker, whereas without visual input no significant att
284                          In contrast, French speakers who had never been exposed to Chinese did not r
285 e birth and differed from monolingual French speakers who had never been exposed to Chinese.
286 igmatic isolated population of Indo-European speakers who have been living for centuries in the Hindu
287 time courses of English speakers and Russian speakers who listened to a real-life Russian narrative a
288 tional magnetic resonance imaging in English speakers who underwent a 12 week intensive French immers
289 MRI data acquired before training in English speakers who underwent a 12 week intensive French immers
290 line activity after the earthquake, Japanese speakers, who are assumed to be more directly affected b
291 ual feedback where they attempted to mimic a speaker whose mouth was seen on an iPod screen; (ii) spe
292 he expectation that interactions with native speakers will provide better opportunities for learning.
293         POPULATION: Literate, native-English speakers with and without AMD.
294                         Even verbally fluent speakers with ASD display distinctive qualities in sign
295    Recent studies reveal that tonal language speakers with autism have enhanced neural sensitivity to
296   Vocal laughter fills conversations between speakers with normal hearing and between deaf users of A
297 n activity was significantly correlated with speakers' with a delay.
298 ides a summary of the data presented by each speaker, with a focus on quantitative techniques and the
299 entations are seen for the speech of the two speakers, with each being selectively phase locked to th
300             This conference brought together speakers working in different areas of cancer research (

WebLSDに未収録の専門用語(用法)は "新規対訳" から投稿できます。
 
Page Top