コーパス検索結果 (1語後でソート)
通し番号をクリックするとPubMedの該当ページを表示します
1 ared with an active control (playing on-line video games).
2 s and by performance tasks such as playing a video game.
3 during a goal-directed motor task, namely a video game.
4 rgical patients playing a virtual-navigation video game.
5 otion discrimination task, and (3) a control video game.
6 gnificant amount of violence in some E-rated video games.
7 have these characteristics simply play more video games.
8 d educational impact similar to robotics and video games.
9 the potential health benefits of active-play video games.
10 ss of three training regimens: (1) an action video game, (2) a psychophysical task that combined atte
13 outcomes that could result from active-play video games and considered how these games could potenti
14 mputer engines that simulate rich physics in video games and graphics, but that uses approximate, pro
15 (CL) sensorimotor interfaces, such as action video games and musical instruments, can impart a broad
16 ough adolescents have embraced the Internet, video games and smart phones, with their extraordinary p
17 the current state of research on active-play video gaming and its potential to serve as a gateway exp
18 -to-end in tasks such as object recognition, video games, and board games, achieving performance that
19 nformal learning environments of television, video games, and the Internet are producing learners wit
20 enue for feature films, first-month sales of video games, and the rank of songs on the Billboard Hot
22 thalamic connectivity changes after use of a video game-based cognitive rehabilitation program, as th
23 ents in the intervention group performed the video game-based cognitive rehabilitation program, while
25 To test the expertise hypothesis, we used a video-game-based paradigm that tacitly trained listeners
26 , to our knowledge, of how a custom-designed video game can be used to assess cognitive abilities acr
27 y stimulating environments within modern-day video games can act as a human correlate of environmenta
28 oved visuomotor skills and whether excessive video gaming causes conduct problems, or whether childre
29 ies are needed to determine whether moderate video gaming causes improved visuomotor skills and wheth
30 f public health concern; however, content in video games commonly played by children has not been qua
31 triatum was significantly reduced during the video game compared with baseline levels of binding, con
32 n than during the resting control and seated video game conditions; however, no significant differenc
35 l in its infancy, results can be surprising: video games designed to be reasonably mindless result in
36 ek spent reading or using a computer/playing video games did not differ between the groups before myo
37 kills can be seen in school children playing video games, even with relatively small amounts of use.
39 t study shows for the first time that action video game experience does indeed improve probabilistic
41 healthy young adults above those of standard video games for measures of brain activity, choice behav
44 energy expenditure when compared with seated video games; however, the influence of AVGs on compensat
45 ut encouraging data on learning outcomes for video games in science, technology, engineering, and mat
46 e debate, the proposed benefits and risks of video gaming in young people remain to be empirically cl
47 hers, and professionals from the active-play video game industry to discuss the potential health bene
49 ly one such intervention being identified: a video game intervention focusing on behavioural issues r
51 ally stimulating virtual environments within video games is a human correlate of environmental enrich
53 social problem is the prevalence of violent video games, leading to desensitization, aggressive beha
54 Daphne Bavelier show that playing an action video game markedly improved subjects performance on a r
55 parents should be aware that popular T-rated video games may be a source of exposure to a wide range
56 rents should understand that popular E-rated video games may be a source of exposure to violence and
60 nd controlled study has found that playing a video game modeled from sensory foraging behavior can im
61 sed with a custom-designed three-dimensional video game (NeuroRacer), exhibits a linear age-related d
63 ernatives: time to spend playing a hand-held video game or time to spend reading magazines or complet
64 g for pleasure, watching television, playing video games or working on the computer), hours per week
67 deo gamers who specifically favor complex 3D video games performed better on a demanding recognition
69 ing demonstrated that the effects of violent video game play are mediated primarily by aggressive cog
72 ers, and we confirm a causal role for action video game play in inducing such improvements through a
75 complex training environments such as action video game play may actually foster brain plasticity and
77 udies have demonstrated an effect of violent video game play on later aggressive behavior, little is
78 mblyopia before and after 40 hours of active video game play, using a rapid serial visual presentatio
80 that certain activities such as gambling or video-game play may be considered addictive in the absen
82 Based on analysis of the 672 current E-rated video games played on home consoles, 77% were in sports,
84 rent aspects of visual attention in habitual video-game players as compared with non-video-game playe
85 lder individuals and preschool children, and video-game players outperform non-players on some tests
88 provides a general mechanism for why action video game playing enhances performance in a wide variet
89 trial), but rather the true effect of action video game playing may be to enhance the ability to lear
93 ontrolling for television and movie viewing, video-game playing, parenting, age, sex, race or ethnici
94 st decade has shown that training on 'action video games' produces learning that transfers well beyon
95 ontent of a convenience sample of 55 E-rated video games released for major home video game consoles
96 naive video gamers in a rich 3D, but not 2D, video game, resulted in a significant improvement in hip
98 experiment, non-players trained on an action video game show marked improvement from their pre-traini
99 und that dyslexic children trained on action video games show significant improvements on basic measu
100 ddition, after 2 weeks of training on the 3D video game Super Mario 3D World, naive video gamers show
102 rom experimental data, and become experts in video games - tasks that would have been deemed impossib
103 ased cognitive training program or web-based video games that do not specifically target executive fu
105 s a significant amount of content in T-rated video games that might surprise adolescent players and t
108 tent descriptors assigned to the 396 T-rated video game titles showed 373 (94%) received content desc
109 Inspired by recent findings that action video game training can impart a broader spectrum of ben
110 ggest that well-documented effects of action video-game training might be due to the sustained deploy
111 ts following attentional tracking and action video-game training suggest that well-documented effects
114 we investigated relationships between weekly video game use, selected cognitive abilities, and conduc
WebLSDに未収録の専門用語(用法)は "新規対訳" から投稿できます。