コーパス検索結果 (1語後でソート)
通し番号をクリックするとPubMedの該当ページを表示します
1 ly involved in decision-making and selective visual attention.
2 a form of competitive selection required for visual attention.
3 ap for control of saccadic eye movements and visual attention.
4 ual stimuli under distributed versus focused visual attention.
5 tion and has been a focus of many studies on visual attention.
6 nificant role individual differences play in visual attention.
7 ed audiovisual attention relative to focused visual attention.
8 d context dependence of socially transmitted visual attention.
9 ints on the functional brain organization of visual attention.
10 entifying the neural mechanisms of selective visual attention.
11 ributed audiovisual attention versus focused visual attention.
12 on that is modulated by perceptual input and visual attention.
13 ature processing interfaces with its role in visual attention.
14 and disrupt the goal-directed allocation of visual attention.
15 ive connectivity of cerebellar crus I during visual attention.
16 efrontal cortex in the bottom-up guidance of visual attention.
17 es, and that the choice process is guided by visual attention.
18 on times, and reduced latencies to disengage visual attention.
19 man subjects as a marker for the strength of visual attention.
20 t saccades and by the visual system to guide visual attention.
21 gly contradictory findings on the effects of visual attention.
22 selection for saccades and the generation of visual attention.
23 key (macaca mulatta) is affected by top-down visual attention.
24 yers outperform non-players on some tests of visual attention.
25 d explain the link between microsaccades and visual attention.
26 ty limitations predicted by most theories of visual attention.
27 ed focus on efficient coding in the field of visual attention.
28 y a key role in controlling the spotlight of visual attention.
29 id not support a high-level understanding of visual attention.
30 other information, a process referred to as visual attention.
31 ortex, and FEF modulates IPS, in relation to visual attention.
32 w details about neuronal interactions during visual attention.
33 graphic variables, miles driven, vision, and visual attention.
34 y deafness causes enhancements in peripheral visual attention.
35 connection between cholinergic signaling and visual attention.
36 increased demands on the limited capacity of visual attention.
37 impairments reflect temporal constraints on visual attention.
38 ns and superior choice accuracy in a test of visual attention.
39 participates in the reward-based control of visual attention.
40 be related to hemispheric specialization for visual attention.
41 ness for unattended events: the dark side of visual attention.
42 rospective cancers did not attract prolonged visual attention.
43 es but does depend on cognitive factors like visual attention.
44 for analyzing the functional neuroanatomy of visual attention.
45 ups, or surfaces, which serve as targets for visual attention.
46 basis for the effects of spatially directed visual attention.
47 Hebbian learning, binaural localization, and visual attention.
48 howing an auditory-phasic alerting effect in visual attention.
49 specialized for the control of feature-based visual attention.
50 ed at the stimulation site, was amplified by visual attention.
51 teractions between cortical loci controlling visual attention.
52 thalamus in the interplay between memory and visual attention.
53 intriguing link between spatial hearing and visual attention.
54 ions, with higher ASSRs during auditory than visual attention.
55 er movement, in analogy to current models of visual attention.
58 ere we report the ipsilateral enhancement of visual attention after repetitive transcranial magnetic
61 f emotional perception and how it relates to visual attention and awareness is likely to require furt
65 the specific prefrontal circuits controlling visual attention and its neural correlates within the pr
69 conscious face perception in particular and visual attention and perceptual awareness in general.
70 udies have explored the neural correlates of visual attention and selection, few have examined the re
72 could not be explained by global changes in visual attention and were specific to value and reward p
75 r colliculus are involved in the guidance of visual attention, and describe the priority map model, w
76 ucidate relationships between eye movements, visual attention, and insight, all of which are employed
79 he functioning of neural networks subserving visual attention, and that these changes are related to
80 ously, TRN activity is modified by shifts of visual attention, and these attentional changes could in
81 relate with psychophysical performance, with visual attention, and with subjective perceptual experie
82 The results show that specific components of visual attention are affected by APOE genotype and that
85 on mechanisms--collectively termed selective visual attention--are guided by intrinsic, bottom-up and
86 ced changes in catecholamine transmission in visual attention areas (eg, occipital and superior parie
88 on of human white matter pathways subserving visual attention, as assessed by diffusion magnetic reso
89 on between bumping while walking and divided visual attention, as measured by the useful field of vie
92 ations, for example, auditory enhancement of visual attention (AV) and visual enhancement of auditory
94 us is suspected to have an important role in visual attention, based on its widespread connectivity w
95 re unveiled, and a representative successful visual attention behavior for each stage of the operatio
96 s gaze response will lead to the transfer of visual attention between crowd members, but it is not su
98 h-sensitive S cones interfere with shifts of visual attention but not with shifts of gaze (saccades).
99 ubjects performed a task requiring shifts of visual attention (but not of gaze) from one location to
100 ditory attention, showing a reduction during visual attention, but no change during auditory attentio
101 arly deafness does not enhance all facets of visual attention, but rather its effects are quite speci
102 cortex are associated with eye movements and visual attention, but their specific contributions are p
103 t, while cholinergic enhancement facilitates visual attention by increasing activity in extrastriate
104 (ACh) and noradrenaline (NA) in arousal and visual attention by simultaneously measuring ACh and NA
105 using a hierarchical approach to determining visual attention, by making an immediate decision based
107 d LTD-like plasticity suggest that voluntary visual attention can exert an important influence on the
108 ration of low-level perceptual responses and visual attention can explain microsaccade rate and direc
112 er a wide range of conditions, including how visual attention changes the gains of neurons in visual
114 hypothesis that the mechanisms implicated in visual attention continue to modulate occipital cortex i
115 V1), and cuneus; neural phase synchrony to a visual attention cue during visual-motor task; and react
121 determine whether the neuronal correlates of visual attention during neutral cueing are similarly int
123 tectable, because they do not draw prolonged visual attention during visual search for breast cancers
129 g/memory, auditory attention/working memory, visual attention/executive function, and speeded languag
132 we trained monkeys to covertly deploy their visual attention from a central fixation point to one of
133 ich participants were required to move their visual attention from face or nonface central fixation s
134 g toddlers had more difficulties disengaging visual attention from faces than toddlers with ASD.
140 Retinotopic borders, motion processing, and visual attention have been the topics of several fMRI st
144 rther elucidate the role of SC in endogenous visual attention, high-resolution fMRI was used to demon
148 results for the first time quantify atypical visual attention in ASD across multiple levels and categ
149 Here, we investigate neural correlates of visual attention in behaving honeybees (Apis mellifera).
150 re we propose neural signatures of selective visual attention in epidural recordings as a fast, relia
151 hat, to understand goal-directed behavior or visual attention in flies, it may be most informative to
152 ts establish changes in different aspects of visual attention in habitual video-game players as compa
153 roach and eye-tracking methodology to assess visual attention in human and macaque infants to faces n
155 ts that the neuronal activity that underlies visual attention in humans is similar to that found in o
159 developmental tests and specialized tests of visual attention in pilot trials of prenatal iodine supp
160 served significant and predictable biases in visual attention in response to both forms of stimulatio
162 ical measurements to study the deployment of visual attention in the brains of humans and nonhuman pr
164 ue perception, intermediate form vision, and visual attention in the upper contralateral visual field
166 are discussed within the scope of models of visual attention in which the pulvinar facilitates commu
167 l search paradigm, which is known to require visual attention, in this study the efficacy of the tech
168 g constructs including selective and divided visual attention, inhibitory control, flexibility, impul
173 re, we ask if this cross-modal plasticity of visual attention is accompanied by an increase in the nu
176 g the preparation of saccadic eye movements, visual attention is confined to the target of intended f
183 In the present study, we investigated how visual attention is shifted during such cuing paradigms
186 formation for online control was achieved by visual attention, its limited capacity [7] would substan
188 There is evidence from the object-based visual attention literature that the FVF may not process
189 educed resolution of spatial and/or temporal visual attention may underlie perceptual deficits relate
191 al proposals that cholinergic enhancement of visual attention might operate via gamma oscillations in
192 imes) but decreased activation in the normal visual attention network (dorsal parietal, bilateral pre
193 rates that sPCS and iPCS fall within a broad visual-attention network, while tgPCS and cIFS fall with
194 pattern in two additional experiments: (i) a visual attention "oddball" task and (ii) a task-free res
195 nce interval 1.30 to 5.63) and impairment in visual attention (odds ratio 2.74; 95% confidence interv
196 visual reorientation behavior and amount of visual attention on individual ROIs in each scenario wer
197 er-surround selection mechanism that focuses visual attention on sensory stimuli also selectively mai
198 ty to motion inputs (e.g., through increased visual attention) or direct scaling of motor outputs (i.
199 ylcholine (ACh) is necessary exclusively for visual attention, or if it also contributes to attention
201 s little change in intelligence quotient and visual attention over the study period, and children's e
202 hanisms that support our ability to modulate visual attention over time, we recorded event-related po
203 03), and trends toward greater impairment on visual attention (P = 0.06) and working memory (P = 0.09
206 contrasts and parallels between auditory and visual attention pathways and mechanisms, the interplay
212 eriments, the computational requirements for visual attention processing are often not clearly presen
213 howing that affective sounds could influence visual attention provide evidence that we make use of af
215 rontal regions during auditory compared with visual attention, putatively reflecting suppression of v
216 a pattern of errors that suggested impaired visual attention rather than a deficiency in perseverati
217 ment of the thalamocortical network, and (2) visual attention reduces the magnitude of alpha oscillat
219 used to determine whether different kinds of visual attention rely on a common neural substrate.
224 cerns show atypicality in the development of visual attention skills from the first year of life.
225 thalamic structures strongly associated with visual attention, specifically the mediodorsal nucleus a
226 re observed in the absence of any changes to visual attention, speed of response, or general motivati
228 aims of this study were to (1) describe the visual attention strategies employed by surgeons that ar
229 m to examine if there are common patterns of visual attention strategies employed by surgeons which a
231 dent activity had several characteristics of visual attention, suggesting that attentional mechanisms
232 ed two bilateral regions that are biased for visual attention, superior precentral sulcus (sPCS) and
233 -motor reaction time (Simple Reaction Time), visual attention (Symbol Digit Substitution), and learni
238 d 23 healthy comparison subjects performed a visual attention task in the presence or absence of dist
239 or 3.0 mg/kg iv), were tested on a 3-choice visual attention task with an olfactory distractor prese
240 other cohort of DAT KD mice was trained on a visual attention task, and no deficits were observed, co
241 ale, and Cech reported that when tested on a visual attention task, the behavior of juvenile chimpanz
242 nd healthy controls while executing a simple visual attention task, we discovered that CBP patients,
247 group counterparts exhibited fewer errors in visual attention/task switching at age 9 years (effect s
248 elligence, attention, attention dysfunction, visual attention/task switching, working memory, and aca
250 with abnormalities in neural response during visual attention tasks in the presence of cross-modal no
251 rrent thermal stimulation (left forearm) and visual attention tasks of titrated difficulty in 20 heal
252 We observed that visual working memory and visual attention tasks robustly recruit cerebellar lobul
254 seronegative [SN] controls) during a set of visual attention tasks with increasing levels of attenti
255 unsupplemented Ts65Dn mice on a series of 5 visual attention tasks, and in fact, on some tasks did n
257 tally were administered a series of 3-choice visual attention tasks, with the most pronounced deficit
258 ietal and occipital brain regions during the visual-attention tasks (all with P < or = 0.001, correct
259 ion-matched control subjects during a set of visual-attention tasks with graded levels of difficulty.
260 rightward shift in the spatial allocation of visual attention, temporarily mimicking spatial deficits
262 mplicated in the neural processes underlying visual attention, the nature of its contribution is not
263 onal oscillations clearly appear integral to visual attention, the role of lower-frequency oscillatio
264 This review will examine two aspects of visual attention: the changes in neural responses within
265 (FEF) are known to represent the position of visual attention, their respective contributions to its
266 ases in affiliative behaviors--lip smacking, visual attention to a caregiver, and time in close proxi
268 correlate of the ability to precisely direct visual attention to locations other than the center of g
269 that frontal lobe damage leads to diminished visual attention to novel events through its disruption
270 This finding indicates an enhancement of visual attention to peripheral visual space in deaf indi
272 entromedial prefrontal cortex damage impairs visual attention to the eye regions of faces, particular
277 with amygdala and fusiform gyrus to modulate visual attention toward motivationally relevant cues.
279 ance was analyzed according to the theory of visual attention (TVA), which provides a computational f
280 parietal cortices to bottom-up and top-down visual attention using electrophysiological measures in
281 Previously, we measured fluctuations in visual attention using the responses of populations of s
283 ntagious behaviors, including propagation of visual attention, violence, opinions, and emotional stat
284 rmation, including visuo-spatial perception, visual attention, visuo-motor transformations and other
285 apture by stimuli that match WM content [7], visual attention was biased toward (task-irrelevant) sti
287 ion of the interplay between WM contents and visual attention was mediated by a neuroanatomical netwo
288 allele of the APOE gene showed deficits when visual attention was spatially directed by cues in tasks
289 identify the neural mechanisms necessary for visual attention, we made restricted lesions, affecting
290 ent-related brain potential (ERP) markers of visual attention, we show that when two targets appear i
291 otential for acetylcholine to play a role in visual attention, we studied nicotinic acetylcholine rec
294 alyzed by Bundesen's computational theory of visual attention, were related to brain metabolism, meas
295 Healthy subjects tend to exhibit a bias of visual attention whereby left hemifield stimuli are proc
297 arent paradox can be resolved by considering visual attention, which is known to enhance basic visual
298 visual cortex during periods of high and low visual attention while participants attended to either t
299 her the use of a sensitive measure of infant visual attention will increase the reliability of such s
WebLSDに未収録の専門用語(用法)は "新規対訳" から投稿できます。