戻る
「早戻しボタン」を押すと検索画面に戻ります。

今後説明を表示しない

[OK]

コーパス検索結果 (1語後でソート)

通し番号をクリックするとPubMedの該当ページを表示します
1 gle or negative-angle rule (depending on the visual stimulus).
2  the same temporal phase relationship to the visual stimulus.
3 TMS alone, without the need for the physical visual stimulus.
4 humans appear to improve many aspects of the visual stimulus.
5 e and negative BOLD associated with a simple visual stimulus.
6  the eyes to make a saccade in response to a visual stimulus.
7 vey its selectivity for the orientation of a visual stimulus.
8 ientation and the direction of motion of the visual stimulus.
9 to elicit escape, even in the absence of any visual stimulus.
10 ed repeated presentations of a nonreinforced visual stimulus.
11 d by cocaine injections and a cocaine-paired visual stimulus.
12 ocal features, respectively, of a reversible visual stimulus.
13  that sound is localized toward a coincident visual stimulus.
14 ex in response to the abrupt appearance of a visual stimulus.
15 n hemoglobin concentrations in response to a visual stimulus.
16 redicts the perceptual fate of a forthcoming visual stimulus.
17 ten observed after the cessation of a bright visual stimulus.
18 late early visual areas in the presence of a visual stimulus.
19  visual areas, but only in the presence of a visual stimulus.
20 nsion will push them away from the expanding visual stimulus.
21 which faces may represent a special class of visual stimulus.
22 nd even if the goal does not correspond to a visual stimulus.
23 visual occipital activity before an expected visual stimulus.
24  than during fixation, despite the lack of a visual stimulus.
25 ally on the spatiotemporal properties of the visual stimulus.
26 tagonistic or integrative depending upon the visual stimulus.
27 e generally to emphasize novel features of a visual stimulus.
28 ts responded to the color or the motion of a visual stimulus.
29 onal activity of the retina in response to a visual stimulus.
30 (CCD) camera in response to a green (540 nm) visual stimulus.
31 with (cued by), but not directed toward, the visual stimulus.
32 , this must be investigated as a function of visual stimulus.
33 rm a spatial cognitive operation on a static visual stimulus.
34  of observed spike responses to a stochastic visual stimulus.
35  influence how long it takes to respond to a visual stimulus.
36 ed time-varying responses to a novel natural visual stimulus.
37 ward (pro-reach) or away (anti-reach) from a visual stimulus.
38 tina to the brain transmit the position of a visual stimulus.
39  energy (local variance in intensity) in the visual stimulus.
40 n which juice was substituted with a neutral visual stimulus.
41  description of how these neurons encode the visual stimulus.
42 s unaffected by the presence or absence of a visual stimulus.
43 utton-presses each produced a specific audio-visual stimulus.
44 onses are self-initiated or in reaction to a visual stimulus.
45 selective for the direction of movement of a visual stimulus.
46 ulation of the cortex with a briefly flashed visual stimulus.
47 revented them from firing in response to the visual stimulus.
48 t alter the classical mislocalization of the visual stimulus.
49 x during (activation) and after (recovery) a visual stimulus.
50 ions in the properties and complexity of the visual stimulus.
51 cts reached away from rather than toward the visual stimulus.
52 redicting the occurrence, timing and type of visual stimulus.
53 es the objective, physical properties of the visual stimulus.
54 ex, influencing processing of a simultaneous visual stimulus.
55 mber of prospects on a branching multivalued visual stimulus.
56 tory target's timecourse matched that of the visual stimulus.
57  is greater if reward is associated with the visual stimulus.
58  when to perform an action following a brief visual stimulus.
59 ed to the specific orientation of the moving visual stimulus.
60 alry by promoting dominance of the congruent visual stimulus.
61  followed by a short lateralized auditory or visual stimulus.
62 xation is modulated by the presentation of a visual stimulus.
63 orded waves of activity elicited by a moving visual stimulus.
64  strongly dependent on the parameters of the visual stimulus.
65  we demonstrate that presentation of a novel visual stimulus (a single oriented grating) causes immed
66 g prolonged presentations of a high-contrast visual stimulus, a process we termed high-contrast adapt
67 ed whether the semantic content of a looming visual stimulus affects perceived time-to-collision by m
68 or orientation as the normal response to the visual stimulus alone.
69 he sound originates from the location of the visual stimulus, an illusion known as the ventriloquism
70 ssociation between the drug and a tactile or visual stimulus and (iii) a test that offers a choice be
71 t included the time required to perceive the visual stimulus and decide which hand to react with; the
72 al flow is modulated upwards on onset of the visual stimulus and downwards, typically with a slower t
73 e other 12 trials the tone was preceded by a visual stimulus and not reinforced.
74 tronger in the hemisphere ipsilateral to the visual stimulus and response hand.
75 xygen level-dependent (BOLD) activity to the visual stimulus and the area responding to the central 3
76 lus, both psychophysical sensitivity to that visual stimulus and the responsiveness of high-order neu
77 specifically, to express the delay between a visual stimulus and the reward that it portends.
78 lation between the conscious perception of a visual stimulus and the synchronous activity of large po
79 lation between the conscious perception of a visual stimulus and the synchronous activity of large po
80 r estimates of the hemodynamic response to a visual stimulus and, under the conditions of a calibrate
81  in activity time-locked to the onset of the visual stimulus, and inhibitory responses.
82 ains depends on the frequency content of the visual stimulus, and that 'relative', not absolute, prec
83  in the direction of the spatially disparate visual stimulus, and the aftereffect did not transfer ac
84 l bases of the interaction between a dynamic visual stimulus approaching the body and its expected co
85 rceptions of the whole and of the parts of a visual stimulus are mediated by different brain regions.
86 fect on pursuit speed and direction when the visual stimulus arises from the coherent motion of a hig
87 re motion or (shape) trajectories before the visual stimulus arrives.
88  where the participants always perceived the visual stimulus as having come first.
89 button-press generated either the same audio-visual stimulus as learned initially, or the pair associ
90 ntion, but we could decode the location of a visual stimulus as well as the endpoint of saccades towa
91 100 ms were temporally linked to an attended visual stimulus, as reflected by robust cross-modal spre
92 at when humans and monkeys were provided the visual stimulus asynchronously with the sound but as fee
93  behavioral task in which they attended to a visual stimulus at one location while remembering a seco
94                        When interrupted by a visual stimulus at random intervals, participants scored
95       The time course of the response to the visual stimulus at the paired RF location is altered, wi
96                 Directing attention toward a visual stimulus at the receptive field of the recorded n
97  sites that encode the retinal location of a visual stimulus before and after a saccade.
98  classes bear different relationships to the visual stimulus, both qualitatively (in terms of the ave
99  not only in the retinotopic location of the visual stimulus, but also at the occipital pole (OP), co
100 ear-nonlinear model that operates not on the visual stimulus, but on the afferent responses of a popu
101 ly enhance behavioral performance based on a visual stimulus, but the degree to which attention modul
102 ust to changes in the temporal contrast of a visual stimulus by imaging activity in vivo.
103                     However, repetition of a visual stimulus can also be considered in terms of the s
104                                    A salient visual stimulus can be rendered invisible by presenting
105  that, during classical fear conditioning, a visual stimulus can be transmitted to the amygdala via e
106 iod between two presentations of an oriented visual stimulus can be used to decode the remembered sti
107 stimulation (TMS) shortly after the end of a visual stimulus can cause a TMS-induced 'replay' or 'vis
108 on in visual cortex and suggest that a prior visual stimulus can influence subsequent perception at e
109                   Repeated experience with a visual stimulus can result in improved perception of the
110 ered architecture of the brain for different visual stimulus categories is one of the most fascinatin
111 ptual expectation effects vary for different visual stimulus categories.
112 lized regions for processing faces and other visual stimulus categories.
113 eraction of Alzheimer's disease severity and visual stimulus complexity in relation to regional brain
114 rvae develop an enhanced motor response to a visual stimulus (conditioned stimulus, CS) when it is pa
115 of the saccade decreased, independent of the visual stimulus configuration.
116  depended on the retinotopic position of the visual stimulus, confirming that the effect was due to t
117 ctile stimuli also promoted dominance of the visual stimulus congruent with the supramodal frequency.
118  of perceptual decisions (independent of the visual stimulus), consistent with a role for MT in provi
119 ere strikingly reproducible across different visual stimulus contexts.
120 nerally showed response suppression when the visual stimulus contrast was high whereas this effect wa
121 nse and this effect was not changed when the visual stimulus contrast was varied.
122                 We further show that, across visual stimulus contrasts, retinal circuits continued to
123  spatial location and orientation of a local visual stimulus coupled to a deep layer of neurons that
124 ht, the initial pause after the onset of the visual stimulus decreased.
125                                      Using a visual stimulus detection task, we provide evidence from
126 ponsive to synaptic burst stimuli, improving visual stimulus detection.
127          Furthermore, we found that the same visual stimulus did not affect performance in auditory c
128                                            A visual stimulus display was created that enabled us to e
129                         We used an immersive visual stimulus dome that allowed us to present spatiote
130 scriminate one of the features that define a visual stimulus (e.g., its orientation) can transfer to
131                          In the absence of a visual stimulus, electrical stimulation of the electrode
132                        The presentation of a visual stimulus elicits a trial-by-trial stimulus-locked
133 so selectively to the spatial structure of a visual stimulus even when there are no energy changes.
134 at a single scalar feature computed from the visual stimulus experienced by the animal is sufficient
135 wn about the neural representation of simple visual stimulus features (for example, orientation, dire
136 s behavior, from the detection of particular visual stimulus features and the timescales of sensory p
137 spatial receptive fields and sharp tuning to visual stimulus features including orientation and spati
138 ate lateral prefrontal cortex (LPFC) encodes visual stimulus features while they are perceived and wh
139 lationships to value, movement direction, or visual stimulus features.
140 representing reward properties together with visual stimulus features.
141 ponse in the hemisphere contralateral to the visual stimulus, followed by a remapped response in the
142 e task that required the crows to remember a visual stimulus for later comparison.
143 subjects are shown an angry face as a target visual stimulus for less than forty milliseconds and are
144 levant sounds occurring synchronously with a visual stimulus from a different location was larger whe
145 d by a saccade as saccadic omission [1]: the visual stimulus generated by the saccade is omitted from
146                    The appearance of a novel visual stimulus generates a rapid stimulus-locked respon
147 was performed on a color monitor driven by a visual-stimulus-generating video board, with stimulus pa
148 during or after presentation of the critical visual stimulus have little or no effect on the subject'
149   We found that exposure to an uninformative visual stimulus (i.e. white light) while simultaneously
150                         However, a change of visual stimulus immediately preceded reach onset, raisin
151 imply as a result of presenting a stationary visual stimulus in and out of the adapted retinal region
152 cotine infusions or a moderately-reinforcing visual stimulus in daily 1-h sessions.
153  between reported visual awareness ("I see a visual stimulus in front of me") and the social attribut
154 itive control on the initial processing of a visual stimulus in humans.
155 more often perceived an inherently ambiguous visual stimulus in one of its perceptual instantiations.
156 ponses are no longer driven primarily by the visual stimulus in the receptive field, but by the broad
157 e (1) highly responsive to the presence of a visual stimulus in the RF, (2) heterogeneous, and (3) no
158 so activity-regulated in vitro or induced by visual stimulus in the visual cortex, suggesting that th
159               The use of video playback as a visual stimulus in this experiment permitted complete is
160 stimulation, OND RGCs respond earlier as the visual stimulus increases in size.
161 e drugs induce characteristic alterations in visual stimulus-induced and/or spontaneous eye movements
162 ctations about the identity of a forthcoming visual stimulus influence the neural mechanisms of perce
163  vector average, but the contribution of the visual stimulus inside the affected field was decreased,
164 ual event can be dramatically reduced if the visual stimulus is accompanied by a startling sound.
165                       Repeated exposure to a visual stimulus is associated with corresponding reducti
166  afterimage induced by prior adaptation to a visual stimulus is believed to be due to bleaching of ph
167 ors affecting the time taken to respond to a visual stimulus is contrast, and studies of reaction tim
168        Analysis of the movement of a complex visual stimulus is expressed in the responses of pattern
169 emovements made in the Wheeless task, when a visual stimulus is followed after a short delay by anoth
170 hows that paying attention to the color of a visual stimulus is manifested by an early endogenous sca
171                  Consequently, when the same visual stimulus is presented repeatedly, postsynaptic cu
172                               When a salient visual stimulus is presented shortly before a saccade, t
173 ivity when a non-informative, suprathreshold visual stimulus is presented, there are highly consisten
174 monstrated that the bottom-up signaling of a visual stimulus is subserved by interareal gamma-band sy
175                                  Because the visual stimulus itself is unchanged, this variability in
176 controlled trial-to-trial differences in the visual stimulus itself, potentially confounding this dis
177 d that their response after the removal of a visual stimulus lasting 1 min was similar in amplitude a
178             Spikes in bipolar cells encode a visual stimulus less reliably than spikes in ganglion ce
179 ts received nonreinforced presentations of a visual stimulus (light) during the 1st training session,
180 Male rats were trained either to associate a visual stimulus (light) with footshock or were exposed t
181 press or completely eliminate responses to a visual stimulus located inside the RF in nitrous oxide s
182 es a continuous representation of remembered visual stimulus locations with respect to constantly cha
183 wing spectral pattern: before the onset of a visual stimulus, low-frequency oscillations (beta, 12-20
184 cyclovergence, its decay in the absence of a visual stimulus may be incomplete.
185  an escape take-off in response to a looming visual stimulus, mimicking a potential predator [8].
186 development have directional preferences for visual stimulus motions; i.e., they give better response
187          The optokinetic response (OKR) to a visual stimulus moving at constant velocity consists of
188 anipulating task difficulty independently of visual stimulus noise, here we test the hypothesis that
189                                          The visual stimulus of a top predator (coral trout, Plectrop
190 a pronounced decrease in their response to a visual stimulus of different contrasts and orientations.
191  in neural activity due to adaptation when a visual stimulus of the same duration was repeatedly pres
192 icient accuracy to precisely reconstruct the visual stimulus on the retina.
193         In this way, we were able to pit the visual stimulus (one vs. two stimulating directions) aga
194                     We found that (1) before visual stimulus onset (-200 to 0 ms), attention to visua
195  applied to left dMFC starting 100 ms before visual stimulus onset and ending 100 ms afterward.
196 raphy to demonstrate that detecting auditory-visual stimulus onset asynchrony activates a large-scale
197 We found that, while the response latency to visual stimulus onset was earlier for V1 neurons than su
198 he incorrect response) starting 180 ms after visual stimulus onset.
199 rsal intraparietal sulcus (IPS) 100 ms after visual stimulus onset.
200 r analyses revealed that eye acceleration at visual-stimulus onset primarily limited working velocity
201 , neurons across visual areas respond to any visual stimulus or contribute to any perceptual decision
202 tent effect when applied before presenting a visual stimulus or during recovery from motion adaptatio
203 upport processing of elemental features of a visual stimulus or scene, such as local contrast, orient
204  conditions used here, such as the choice of visual stimulus or spike measurement time window, and th
205 lation did not depend on the presence of the visual stimulus or the behavioral choice of the animal.
206 tion is focused on an object such as a small visual stimulus or the breath.
207                               Selectivity to visual stimulus orientation is a basic cortical function
208 f neuronal responses to the orientation of a visual stimulus (orientation tuning).
209  Two measures of conditioned responding to a visual stimulus, orienting behavior (rearing on the hind
210   Monkeys made saccades in the presence of a visual stimulus outside of the receptive field.
211 perception when the evoked phosphene and the visual stimulus overlapped in time and space in the plac
212 ould care about an almost infinite number of visual stimulus patterns, but we don't: we distinguish t
213  stronger than any suppression evoked by the visual stimulus per se.
214  introduce trial-to-trial variability in the visual stimulus, potentially confounding such measures.
215 ning engaged upon first encountering a novel visual stimulus predicts the degree of perceptual fluenc
216  Transient pupil dilation was elicited after visual stimulus presentation regardless of target lumina
217 ee epochs of the memory-guided saccade task: visual stimulus presentation, the delay interval, and mo
218  neurons in 24 neurosurgical patients during visual stimulus presentation.
219 ite during naming started 250-300 msec after visual stimulus presentation.
220 wn that the responses of many Ipc units to a visual stimulus presented inside the classical receptive
221  lifting a finger or an arm in response to a visual stimulus presented to either hemifield, this acal
222 d pattern of activation corresponding to the visual stimulus presented.
223  concurrent multiunit firing and facilitates visual stimulus processing.
224                              What parts of a visual stimulus produce the greatest neural signal?
225                                          The visual stimulus produced statistically significant negat
226        We show that closed-loop control of a visual stimulus promotes temporal coordination across th
227 ms of groups of neurons (channels) tuned for visual stimulus properties.
228 ways, in a manner that can be dependent upon visual stimulus properties.
229 nglion cells in the mouse retina over a wide visual stimulus range.
230  on anti-reaches encoded the location of the visual stimulus rather than the movement goal.
231            Here, we found that brief (50 ms) visual stimulus re-exposure during a repetitive foil tas
232               Shifting attention away from a visual stimulus reduces, but does not abolish, visual di
233 ral mechanisms leading to the awareness of a visual stimulus remain unclear.
234                        In the main task, the visual stimulus remained constant, but covert visual spa
235 was that neurons in the FEF report whether a visual stimulus remains stable or moves as a saccade is
236 e focused on identifying which features of a visual stimulus render it salient--i.e., make it "pop ou
237 , two GCaMP5 variants detected twice as many visual stimulus-responsive cells as GCaMP3.
238 hat the task-relevant feature of an upcoming visual stimulus (S2) was, while high-density electroence
239 ain neurons implicated in navigation-display visual stimulus selection.
240 s by which spike frequency adaptation shapes visual stimulus selectivity in an identified visual inte
241 following repetitive presentation of a given visual stimulus, spatiotemporal activity patterns resemb
242 pants directed their attention to one of two visual stimulus streams located adjacent to each hand.
243 tended selectively to one of two lateralized visual-stimulus streams while task-irrelevant tones were
244  presented human observers with an ambiguous visual stimulus (structure-from-motion) that can give ri
245  influenced by the low-level salience of the visual stimulus, such as luminance, contrast, and color,
246                                       When a visual stimulus suddenly appears, it captures attention,
247 umans lengthened the perceived duration of a visual stimulus, suggesting the rSMG is involved in basi
248       Initiating a movement in response to a visual stimulus takes significantly longer than might be
249            In phase 1 of the DMST, a complex visual stimulus (target) was followed immediately by a f
250 he modes represent localized features of the visual stimulus that are distinct from the features repr
251  is played by a common representation of the visual stimulus that can be applied at different stages.
252 tterns represent specific messages about the visual stimulus that differ significantly from what one
253                        In natural viewing, a visual stimulus that is the target of attention is gener
254 g theory to train a discrimination between a visual stimulus that predicted reward (conditioned excit
255  and extent of switching a response toward a visual stimulus that was previously not, but has become,
256 ome, associated with reward (and away from a visual stimulus that was previously, but is no longer, r
257 t for encoding global motion, we developed a visual stimulus that yields a global direction yet inclu
258      The task employs a continuously varying visual stimulus that, for any moment in time, selectivel
259 ntity versus attractiveness) within the same visual stimulus (the face).
260 itations of TMS-induced replay with the same visual stimulus, the replay can be induced by TMS alone,
261 le, when monkeys direct their attention to a visual stimulus, the response gain of specific subsets o
262 ngle of polarization of a linearly polarized visual stimulus, thereby maximizing the polarization con
263                                              Visual stimulus to be detected was a temporally modulate
264 the efficiency of disengaging from a central visual stimulus to orient to a peripheral one in a cohor
265 surprise associated with the occurrence of a visual stimulus to provide a formal quantification of th
266 show spontaneous oddity preference for all 3 visual stimulus types tested (photos, shapes, and patter
267 s responded very differently to an identical visual stimulus under different visual discrimination ta
268 hen attention is deployed on a high-contrast visual stimulus using a discrimination task.
269 he neural activity evoked by an extinguished visual stimulus, using event-related functional MRI (fMR
270 s to mislocalization of the sound toward the visual stimulus (ventriloquism illusion).
271      In order to direct a movement towards a visual stimulus, visual spatial information must be comb
272 ral SC deactivation, orienting to the static visual stimulus was abolished throughout the entire visu
273                                     When the visual stimulus was absent or had low contrast, these LF
274 t location was larger when that accompanying visual stimulus was attended versus unattended.
275 he asynchronous condition, but only when the visual stimulus was body related.
276            Around the time of the saccade, a visual stimulus was flashed either at the location occup
277 ficantly predicted whether a threshold-level visual stimulus was later consciously perceived.
278  However, the proportion of V1 active to our visual stimulus was lower for the older observers than f
279 g behavior (rearing on the hind legs) when a visual stimulus was paired with food.
280 ed rats exhibited normal responding when the visual stimulus was presented alone and paired with food
281                         In contrast when the visual stimulus was presented synchronously with the sou
282                                            A visual stimulus was presented to the eye, and responses
283 the left or right hand to indicate whether a visual stimulus was relatively large or small.
284 , we found previously that when a flickering visual stimulus was repeated, individual cells fired act
285 food on some trials, while on other trials a visual stimulus was simultaneously presented along with
286                          The strength of the visual stimulus was titrated around psychophysical thres
287 both in the presence and in the absence of a visual stimulus, was a tonic hyperpolarization.
288 spread," i.e., impulse response to a minimal visual stimulus, was as rapid and retinotopically specif
289                    When we repeated the same visual stimulus, we found that the same mode was robustl
290 lty in learning to correct their choice of a visual stimulus when it is no longer associated with rew
291 e enhancement of attentional processing of a visual stimulus when its predictive value was altered.
292        Subjects made decisions about a noisy visual stimulus, which they indicated by moving a handle
293 red light in the retinal region exposed to a visual stimulus, which was significant in three of five
294  Following one action the audio preceded the visual stimulus, while for the other action audio lagged
295 ortex are selective for the orientation of a visual stimulus, while the receptive fields of their tha
296 ssion by following a large-field, patterned, visual stimulus with a magnetic pulse.
297 rategy that links the luminance profile in a visual stimulus with an association (the percept) that r
298 ), we show that association of a directional visual stimulus with reward results in broadened orienta
299 s whose amplitude varied independently and a visual stimulus with varying radius, while manipulating
300 ated whether changing the probability that a visual stimulus would be selected as the target for a sa

WebLSDに未収録の専門用語(用法)は "新規対訳" から投稿できます。
 
Page Top